Skip to main content
Log in

Temporal Closeness Promotes Imitation of Meaningful Gestures in Face-to-Face Communication

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present research investigated how temporal distance affects imitation of gestures during a conversation. According to construal level theory, psychological closeness (vs. distance) leads to preference for more pictorial and embodied (vs. symbolic and amodal) forms of communication as well as to greater context sensitivity. We thus hypothesized that people are more sensitive to the contextual meaning of gestures and show more imitation of meaningful (vs. meaningless) gestures when primed with temporal closeness (compared to distance). Participants (N = 91) were assigned to the role of a job interviewer for an internship that would begin in 1 week (vs. in 1 year) and interacted with an ostensible applicant who displayed various meaningful and meaningless gestures. When the internship was to start in 1 week, participants imitated meaningful gestures more often than meaningless gestures. When the internship was to start in 1 year, participants imitated both gesture types equally frequently. This finding indicates that people are more likely to embody verbal statements of an interaction partner when feeling psychologically close to the subject of the conversation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In a pretest in which we did not explicitly instruct participants to imitate the other’s nonverbal expressions we were not able to find sufficiently high baseline rates of imitation. We manipulated participants’ mindset via the how-why task (Alter et al. 2010; Freitas et al. 2004) and presented two videos of a model displaying either meaningless (e.g., neck rolling in a scene in which someone prepares herself in front of a mirror) or meaningful (e.g., neck rolling in scene in which someone is doing yoga) behaviors for the situation. Only 30% of participants mimicked at least one of the gestures overall. This very low baseline rate did not allow for any further tests of moderators. This is why we developed the job interview paradigm with an explicit instruction to imitate. A second reason for instructing participants explicitly was to keep motivational influences and the rate of spontaneous gesturing as low as possible.

  2. The two raters also coded the accuracy of the gestural imitations (on a scale from 0 = was not accurately imitated at all to 9 = was imitated entirely and accurately) and their duration in seconds. A 2 × 2 mixed ANOVA on the accuracy of imitated gestures revealed a main effect of gesture type, F(1, 83) = 12.13, p = .001, η 2p  = .13. Meaningful gestures (M = 0.99, SD = 0.86) were imitated more accurately than meaningless gestures (M = 0.78, SD = 0.67). There was no interaction between gesture type and distance, F < 1, but a marginal main effect of distance, F(1, 83) = 2.95, p = .09, η 2p  = .03. In the close condition, participants were more accurate (M = 1.01, SD = 0.11) than in the distal condition (M = 0.74, SD = 0.11), replicating previous research on the effect of distance on exactness of imitation (Hansen et al. 2016). A 2 × 2 mixed ANOVA on the duration of imitated gestures revealed no main effect of gesture type, F(1, 83) = 2.73, p = .102, and no interaction between gesture type and duration, F < 1, but a marginally significant main effect of distance, F(1, 83) = 3.79, p = .055, η 2p  = .04. In the close condition, participants imitated the gestures longer (M = 0.61, SD = 0.07) than in the distal condition (M = 0.42, SD = 0.07).

  3. While the interviewer was reading the first, second, and third question to the confederate, we could also observe if s/he spontaneously used one of the 12 gestures before the confederate used them for the first time. For nine out of the 12 gestures, no participant used them spontaneously. Three gestures (i.e., rolling hands, tapping with one hand on the table, and opening hands) were spontaneously used by 2.4% of participants (n = 2), respectively. As these were very low baseline rates of spontaneous behavior, we did not include this as a control variable.

  4. Since the number of imitated meaningful and meaningless gestures significantly correlated with interview duration in minutes (cf. Table 2), we conducted the same analysis using interview duration as a covariate. When controlling for interview duration, the interaction between gesture and distance remained significant, F(1, 82) = 4.07, p = .05, η 2p  = .05. We also conducted the main analysis with the whole sample without exclusions (N = 91). For the number of imitated gestures, a 2 × 2 mixed ANOVA revealed a main effect of gesture, F(1, 89) = 8.01, p = .006, η 2p  = .08. Meaningful gestures (M = 6.30, SD = 5.21) were imitated more often than meaningless gestures (M = 5.12, SD = 4.97), M D = 1.18, SE D = 0.42, 95% CI [0.36, 2.01]. There was no main effect of distance, F(1, 89) = 1.06, p = .31, η 2p  = .01. However, in line with the hypothesis, a marginally significant interaction between gesture and distance emerged, F(1, 89) = 3.43, p = .07, η 2p  = .04. Simple effects revealed that in the close distance condition, participants imitated the meaningful (M = 7.22, SD = 5.87) compared to meaningless (M = 5.27, SD = 4.69) gestures significantly more often, M D = 1.96, SE D = 0.59, 95% CI [0.78, 3.13], p = .001. In the distant condition, however, participants imitated meaningful (M = 5.39, SD = 5.29) gestures as often as meaningless (M = 4.98, SD = 5.27) ones, M D = 0.41, SE D = 0.59, 95% CI [–0.75, 1.58], p = .48. No other comparisons were significant, p s  > .12.

References

  • Alter, A. L., Oppenheimer, D. M., & Zemla, J. C. (2010). Missing the trees for the forest: A construal level account of the illusion of explanatory depth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(3), 436–451. doi:10.1037/a0020218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Amit, E., Algom, D., & Trope, Y. (2009a). Distance-dependent processing of pictures and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(3), 400–415. doi:10.1037/a0015835.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amit, E., Algom, D., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2009b). “Thou shalt not make unto thee any graven image”: The distance dependence of representation. In K. D. Markman, W. M. P. Klein, & J. A. Suhr (Eds.), Handbook of imagination and mental simulation (pp. 53–68). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amit, E., Wakslak, C., & Trope, Y. (2012). The use of visual and verbal means of communication across psychological distance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39(1), 43–56. doi:10.1177/0146167212460282.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(4), 596–612. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W., Niedenthal, P. M., Barbey, A. K., & Ruppert, J. A. (2003). Social embodiment. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 43, pp. 43–92). New York, NY: Elsevier Science. doi:10.1017/S0140525X99002149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cassell, J., McNeill, D., & McCullough, K.-E. (1999). Speech-gesture mismatches: Evidence for one underlying representation of linguistic and nonlinguistic information. Pragmatics & Cognition, 7(1), 1–34. doi:10.1075/pc.7.1.03cas.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, C. M., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Self-monitoring without awareness: Using mimicry as a nonconscious affiliation strategy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(6), 1170–1179. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10(85). http://www.citeulike.org/group/7057/article/3410177

  • Dusthimer, N. E., Ameigh, A. L., Dickey, K. J., & Hall, C. E. (2015). The influence on construal level on behavioral mimicry. Poster presented at the 16th annual convention of the society for personality and social psychology, Long Beach (CA), USA.

  • Feyereisen, P., Van de Wiele, M., & Dubois, F. (1988). The meaning of gestures: What can be understood without speech? Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition, 8(1), 3–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freitas, A. L., Gollwitzer, P., & Trope, Y. (2004). The influence of abstract and concrete mindsets on anticipating and guiding others’ self-regulatory efforts. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(6), 739–752. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2004.04.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fujita, K., Henderson, M. D., Eng, J., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2006a). Spatial distance and mental construal of social events. Psychological Science, 17(4), 278–282. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01698.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fujita, K., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Levin-Sagi, M. (2006b). Construal levels and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 351–367. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.3.351.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fujita, K., & Roberts, J. (2010). Promoting prospective self-control through abstraction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 1049–1054. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.05.013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S. (1999). The role of gesture in communication and thinking. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(11), 419–429. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01397-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gonzaga, G. C., Keltner, D., Londahl, E. A., & Smith, M. D. (2001). Love and the commitment problem in romantic relations and friendship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(2), 247–262. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gullberg, M., & Holmqvist, K. (1999). Keeping an eye on gestures: Visual perception of gestures in face-to-face communication. Pragmatics & Cognition, 7(1), 35–63. doi:10.1075/pc.7.1.04gul.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hansen, J., Alves, H., & Trope, Y. (2016). Psychological distance reduces literal imitation: Evidence from an imitation-learning paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(3), 320–330. doi:10.1037/xhp0000150.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, M. D., & Wakslak, C. J. (2010). Psychological distance and priming: When do semantic primes impact social evaluations? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(7), 975–985. doi:10.1177/0146167210367490.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holler, J., & Wilkin, K. (2011). Co-speech gesture mimicry in the process of collaborative referring during face-to-face dialogue. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 35(2), 133–153. doi:10.1007/s10919-011-0105-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hostetter, A. B. (2011). When do gestures communicate? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 137(2), 297–315. doi:10.1037/a0022128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. (2008). Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(15), 495–514. doi:10.3758/PBR.15.3.495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2010). Language, gesture, action! A test of the gesture as simulated action framework. Journal of Memory and Language, 63(2), 245–257. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2010.04.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hostetter, A. B., Alibali, M. W., & Niedenthal, P. M. (2012). Embodied social thought: Linking social concepts, emotion, and gesture. In S. Fiske & C. N. Macrae (Eds.), SAGE handbook of social cognition (pp. 211–228). London: Sage Publications.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kimbara, I. (2006). On gestural mimicry. Gesture, 6(1), 39–61. doi:10.1075/gest.6.1.03kim.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lammers, J. (2012). Abstraction increases hypocrisy. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 475–480. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2011.07.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (1998). The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(1), 5–18. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.1.5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (2008). The psychology of transcending the here and now. Science, 322(5905), 1201–1205. doi:10.1126/science.1161958.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Liviatan, I., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2008). Interpersonal similarity as a social distance dimension: Implications for perception of others’ actions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(5), 1256–1269. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.04.007.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Maddux, W. W., Mullen, E., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Chameleons bake bigger pies and take bigger pieces: Strategic behavioral mimicry facilitates negotiation outcomes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(2), 461–468. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2007.02.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maglio, S. J., & Trope, Y. (2012). Disembodiment: Abstract construal attenuates the influence of contextual bodily state in judgment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(2), 211–216. doi:10.1037/a0024520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malkoc, S. A., Zauberman, G., & Bettman, J. R. (2010). Unstuck from the concrete: Carryover effects of abstract mindsets in intertemporal preferences. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 113, 112–126. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2010.07.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mol, L., Krahmer, E., Maes, A., & Swerts, M. (2012). Adaptation in gesture: Converging hands or converging minds? Journal of Memory and Language, 66(1), 249–264. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2011.07.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rauscher, F. H., Krauss, R. M., & Chen, Y. (1996). Gesture, speech, and lexical access: The role of lexical movements in speech production. Psychological Science, 7(4), 226–231. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00364.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenfeld, H. M. (1966). Instrumental affiliative functions of facial and gestural expressions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4(1), 65–72. doi:10.1037/h0023514.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Semin, G. R. (2007). Grounding communication: Synchrony. In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 630–649). New York: Guilford Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shapira, O., Liberman, N., Trope, Y., & Rim, S. (2012). Levels of mental construal. In S. Fiske & C. N. Macrae (Eds.), Sage handbook of social cognition (pp. 229–250). London: Sage Publications.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, L. E., Michikyan, M., & Greenfield, P. M. (2013). The effects of text, audio, video, and in-person communication on bonding between friends. Cyberpsychology Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace, 7(2), 1–12. doi:10.5817/CP2013-2-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stel, M., Van Baaren, R. B., & Vonk, R. (2008). Effects of mimicking: Acting prosocially by being emotionally moved. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38(6), 965–976. doi:10.1002/ejsp.472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, C. K., & Kristof, A. L. (1995). Making the right impression: A field study of applicant impression management during job interviews. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80(5), 587–606. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.80.5.587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117(2), 440–463. doi:10.1037/a0018963.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1989). Levels of personal agency: Individual variation in action identification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(4), 660–671. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.4.660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wakslak, C. J., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Alony, R. (2006). Seeing the forest when entry is unlikely: Probability and the mental representation of events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(4), 641–653. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.135.4.641.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Austrian Science Fund (FWF; Grant No. P25307-G22) awarded to Jochim Hansen. We wish to thank Sophie-Therés Graffius for help conducting the studies, Garrit Jentsch and Josefine Sirin Gökbayrak for their help with the speech analysis, and Mary-Louise Grossman for proof-reading the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Janet Wessler.

Appendices

Appendix 1: Pretest of Gestures

We developed a fictional job interview with six critical questions and answers. The pretest was designed to validate the meaningfulness of the scripted gestures accompanying each answer. We video-recorded a confederate answering the six interview questions together with corresponding scripted gestures. In each case, the confederate used a total of four gestures, two meaningful and two meaningless ones (see Table 1), which were arranged a priori in a fixed quasi-randomized order. For the first three answers, the confederate used 12 different gestures: six meaningful and six meaningless. For the remaining three answers, she used the same 12 gestures but in their meaningless version (when the gesture was meaningful before) and meaningful version (when the gesture was meaningless before), respectively. To keep the visual field of gestures constant, all gestures occurred within the physical space of the confederate’s body center (Gullberg and Holmqvist 1999; McNeill 1992). The confederate was trained to perform both versions of a gesture equally often, intense, and long, independent of her accompanying verbal statement.

For the pretest, 31 undergraduate psychology students were randomly assigned to one of two distance conditions (close: n = 15 in 1 week vs. distant: n = 16 in 1 year). As shown in Table 1, distance was manipulated by the phrase “in 1 week” (“in 1 year”) which was included in each interview question. Participants were asked to read each of the six interview questions on a computer screen and watch the applicant’s respective answer. For each of the 24 gestures that participants watched during the whole interview, they answered five items on a paper–pencil questionnaire in front of them. Four items asked participants to rate how important, meaningful, appropriate, and incidental the gesture was in the given situation, according to a 9-point Likert-scale (1 = not at all to 9 = very much). One item asked participants how likely they would spontaneously imitate the gesture in a real interview if they were the job interviewer. In (re)viewing the video to answer the items, participants could stop, fast forward, or reverse the track as often as they liked.

We found satisfactory internal reliability between the three items measuring the meaningfulness of the gestures (important, meaningful, appropriate; Cronbach’s alphas from .63 to .97), and thus calculated the mean of these three items for the meaningful and meaningless gestures as a measure of meaningfulness. One item (“How incidental is the gesture in the situation?”) substantially reduced the reliability of the meaningfulness scale and was excluded. There was no difference between meaningful and meaningless gestures on this item, nor an interaction with condition, Fs < 1.36.

For the meaningfulness measure, a 2 (Distance: in 1 week vs. in 1 year) × 2 (Meaning of gesture: meaningful vs. meaningless) mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed a main effect of meaning, F(1, 29) = 116.32, p < .001, η 2p  = .80, f = 2.00. Participants rated gestures that emphasized the verbal statement of the applicant as significantly more meaningful (M = 6.33, SD = 1.08) than gestures that did not (M = 4.19, SD = 0.94), M D = 2.14, SE D = 0.20, 95% CI [1.74, 2.55]. There was neither a main effect of distance nor a significant interaction between meaning of gesture and distance, Fs < 1.

For the self-reported likelihood of imitating the gesture in a real interview, a 2 (Distance: in 1 week vs. 1 year) × 2 (Meaning of gesture: meaningful vs. meaningless) mixed ANOVA also revealed a main effect of meaningfulness, F(1, 29) = 61.10, p < .001, η 2p  = .68, f = 1.46. Participants reported imitating meaningful gestures with a significantly higher likelihood (M = 47.10%, SD = 19.82) than meaningless gestures (M = 26.16%, SD = 12.03). There was neither a main effect of distance nor a significant interaction between meaningfulness and distance, Fs < 1. Ratings for the likelihood of gestural imitation were not influenced by the distance manipulation.

In summary, by varying the accompanying verbal statement in our study design we could successfully create a meaningless versus a meaningful version of each gesture. In the main study, participants and the confederate engaged in a simulation of a real job interview to investigate actual imitation behavior.

Appendix 2: Post Interview Questionnaire

The final (exploratory) questionnaire contained two items about the evaluation of the applicant (i.e., “How likely is it that you as human resources manager would employ the person?”, “How good do you think the applicant’s answers were?”, r = .76, p < .001), one item about the extent to which they thought the applicant’s gestures fit what she was saying, five items about how much they liked the applicant (e.g., “How much do you like the person?”, “How close did you feel to the person?”, Cronbach’s α = .85), five items about their motivation during the interview (“How interesting was the interview?”, “How motivated were you to conduct the interview well?”, Cronbach’s α = .78), three items about their mood during the interview (e.g., “Did you feel comfortable during the interview?”, Cronbach’s α = .90), and seven items from the perspective-taking sub-scale of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI; Davis 1980; e.g., “Before criticizing somebody, I try to imagine how I would feel if I were in their place”, Cronbach’s α = .73). Participants rated all of these items on a 9-point Likert scale (1 = not at all to 9 = very much). In addition, participants indicated the extent to which they felt socially close to the applicant via the Inclusion of the Other in the Self Scale (IOS; Aron et al. 1992). One item asked which kind of tasks they would assign to the applicant if she started the internship (from making copies to tasks with more responsible such as customer care). For correlations between the study variables with the temporal distance manipulation and the main dependent variable frequency of imitation, see Table 2 in Appendix 2.

Table 2 Pearson’s Product Moment Correlation Matrix Between the Distance Manipulation and all Measured Variables (Behavioral Identification Form as a measure of level of construal, mean number of imitated meaningless and meaningful gestures, overall post-interview evaluation of the applicant, liking of the applicant (the confederate), self-reported perspective taking measured with IRI subscale (Davis 1980), interviewers’ self- reported motivation during the interview, self-reported mood during the interview, overall interview duration in seconds)

Afterwards, participants filled out a modified version of the Behavioral Identification Form (BIF; Vallacher and Wegner 1989) as explained in the main text body. Finally, an open-ended question probed if participants correctly recollected the time point for the start of the internship. Six participants were not able to freely recall when the applicant would have started her internship (i.e., in 1 week or in 1 year), and were therefore excluded from further analyses (n = 2 in 1 week; n = 4 in 1 year), which was correctly remembered by 93.4% of the participants (close condition: 95.6%; distance condition: 91.3%). At the end, they were asked to report their age, sex, education, and any suspicion they had regarding the experimental procedure and purpose of the study. No participant correctly guessed the purpose of the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wessler, J., Hansen, J. Temporal Closeness Promotes Imitation of Meaningful Gestures in Face-to-Face Communication. J Nonverbal Behav 41, 415–431 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-017-0256-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-017-0256-1

Keywords

Navigation