Algoe, S. B., Buswell, B. N., & DeLamater, J. D. (2000). Gender and job status as contextual cues for the interpretation of facial expression of emotion. Sex Roles,
42(3), 183–208. doi:10.1023/A:1007087106159.
Article
Google Scholar
Becker, D. V., Kenrick, D. T., Neuberg, S. L., Blackwell, K. C., & Smith, D. M. (2007). The confounded nature of angry men and happy women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
92(2), 179–190. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.2.179.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bijlstra, G., Holland, R. W., & Wigboldus, D. H. J. (2010). The social face of emotion recognition: Evaluations versus stereotypes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
46, 657–663. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.006.
Article
Google Scholar
Brescoll, V. L., & Uhlmann, E. L. (2008). Can an angry woman get ahead? Status conferral, gender, and expression of emotion in the workplace. Psychological Science,
19(3), 268–275. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02079.x.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Brody, L. R., & Hall, J. A. (2008). Gender and emotion in context. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (Vol. 3, pp. 395–408). New York: Guilford Press.
Google Scholar
Brody, L. R., Lovas, G. S., & Hay, D. H. (1995). Gender differences in anger and fear as a function of situational context. Sex Roles,
32(1–2), 47–48. doi:10.1007/BF01544757.
Article
Google Scholar
Carney, D., Hall, J. A., & Smith LeBeau, L. (2005). Beliefs about the nonverbal expression of social power. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,
29(2), 105–123. doi:10.1007/s10919-005-2743-z.
Article
Google Scholar
Condry, J., & Condry, S. (1976). Sex differences: A study of the eye of the beholder. Child Development,
47(3), 812–819.
Google Scholar
Dépret, E., & Fiske, S. T. (1999). Perceiving the powerful: Intriguing individuals versus threatening groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
35(5), 461–480. doi:10.1006/jesp.1999.1380.
Article
Google Scholar
Dovidio, J. F., & Fazio, R. H. (1992). New technologies for the direct and indirect assessment of attitudes. In J. M. Tanur (Ed.), Questions about questions: Inquiries into the cognitive bases of surveys (pp. 204–237). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.
Google Scholar
Eagly, A. H., & Carli, L. L. (2007). Through the labyrinth: The truth about how women become leaders. Boston: Harvard Business School Press.
Google Scholar
Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1984). Gender stereotypes stem from the distribution of women and men into social roles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
46(4), 735–754. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.46.4.735.
Article
Google Scholar
Eagly, A. H., Makhijani, M. G., & Klonsky, B. G. (1992). Gender and the evaluation of leaders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin,
111(1), 3–22. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.111.1.3.
Article
Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1976). The Pictures of Facial Affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Google Scholar
Everhart, D. E., & Harrison, D. W. (2000). Facial affect perception in anxious and nonanxious men without depression. Psychobiology,
28(1), 90–98.
Google Scholar
Fabes, R. A., & Martin, C. L. (1991). Gender and age stereotypes of emotionality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
17(5), 532. doi:10.1177/0146167291175008.
Article
Google Scholar
Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
69(6), 1013. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1013.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hall, J. A., Coats, E. J., & Smith LeBeau, L. (2005). Nonverbal behavior and the vertical dimension of social relations: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin,
131(6), 898–924. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.89.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hecht, M. A., & LaFrance, M. (1998). License or obligation to smile: The effect of power and sex on amount and type of smiling. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
24(12), 1332. doi:10.1177/01461672982412007.
Article
Google Scholar
Hess, U., Senécal, S., Kirouac, G., Herrera, P., Philippot, P., & Kleck, R. E. (2000). Emotional expressivity in men and women: Stereotypes and self-perceptions. Cognition and Emotion,
14(5), 609–642. doi:10.1080/02699930050117648.
Article
Google Scholar
Hess, U., Adams, R. B., Grammer, K., & Kleck, R. E. (2009). Face gender and emotion expression: Are angry women more like men? Journal of Vision, 9(12), 19, 1–8. doi:10.1167/9.12.19.
Hugenberg, K. (2005). Social categorization and the perception of facial affect: Target race moderates the response latency advantage for happy faces. Emotion,
5(3), 267–276. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.3.267.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hugenberg, K., & Sczesny, S. (2006). On wonderful women and seeing smiles: Social categorization moderates the happy face response latency advantage. Social Cognition,
24(5), 516–539. doi:10.1521/soco.2006.24.5.516.
Article
Google Scholar
Johnson, K. L., Freeman, J. B., & Pauker, K. (2011). Race is gendered: How covarying phenotypes and stereotypes bias sex categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. doi:10.1037/a0025335.
Kring, A. M. (2000). Gender and anger. In A. H. Fischer (Ed.), Gender and emotion: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 211–231). Edinburgh: Cambridge University Press.
Chapter
Google Scholar
LaFrance, M., & Banaji, M. (1992). Toward a reconsideration of the gender-emotion relationship. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Emotion and social behavior (pp. 178–201). Newbury Park: Sage.
Google Scholar
LaFrance, M., Hecht, M. A., & Paluck, E. L. (2003). The contingent smile: A meta-analysis of sex differences in smiling. Psychological Bulletin,
129(2), 305–334. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.30.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Lewis, K. M. (2000). When leaders display emotion: How followers respond to negative emotional expression of male and female leaders. Journal of Organizational Behavior,
21(2), 221–234. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(200003)21:2<221:AID-JOB36>3.0.CO;2-0.
Article
Google Scholar
MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin,
109, 163–203. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Maner, J. K., DeWall, C. N., & Gailliot, M. T. (2008). Selective attention to signs of success: Social dominance and early stage interpersonal perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
34(4), 488–501. doi:10.1177/0146167207311910.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Maybury, K. K. (1997). The influence of status and sex on observer judgements of anger displays (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of California, Davis.
Moretti, M. M., Charlton, S., & Taylor, S. (1996). The effects of hemispheric asymmetries and depression on the perception of emotion. Brain and Cognition,
32(1), 67–82.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Plant, E. A., Hyde, J. S., Keltner, D., & Devine, P. G. (2000). The gender stereotyping of emotions. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
24(1), 81–92. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2000.tb01024.x.
Article
Google Scholar
Plant, E. A., Kling, K. C., & Smith, G. L. (2004). The influence of gender and social role on the interpretation of facial expressions. Sex Roles,
51(3–4), 187–196. doi:10.1023/B:SERS.0000037762.10349.13.
Article
Google Scholar
Ratcliff, R. (1979). Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. Psychological Bulletin,
86(3), 446–461.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ratcliff, R. (1993). Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychological Bulletin,
114(3), 510. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.114.3.510.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ratcliff, N. J., Franklin, R. G, Jr, Nelson, A. J., & Vescio, T. K. (2012). The scorn of status: A bias toward perceiving anger on high-status faces. Social Cognition,
30(5), 631–642. doi:10.1521/soco.2012.30.5.631.
Article
Google Scholar
Ridgeway, C. L. (2001). Gender, status, and leadership. Journal of Social Issues,
57(4), 637–655. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00233.
Article
Google Scholar
Rotter, N. G., & Rotter, G. S. (1988). Sex differences in the encoding and decoding of negative facial emotions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,
12(2), 139–148.
Article
Google Scholar
Rudman, L. A., Moss-Racusin, C. A., Phelan, J. E., & Nauts, S. (2012). Status incongruity and backlash effects: Defending the gender hierarchy motivates prejudice against female leaders. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
48(1), 165–179. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2011.10.008.
Article
Google Scholar
Stroessner, S. J. (1996). Social categorization by race or sex: Effects of perceived non-normalcy on response times. Social Cognition,
14(3), 247–276. doi:10.1521/soco.1996.14.3.247.
Article
Google Scholar
Thomas, E. L., Dovidio, J. F., & West, T. V. (2014). Lost in the categorical shuffle: Evidence for the social non-prototypicality of Black women. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology,
20(3), 370–376. doi:10.1037/a0035096.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Tiedens, L. Z., Ellsworth, P. C., & Mesquita, B. (2000). Sentimental stereotypes: Emotional expectations for high- and low-status group members. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
26(5), 560. doi:10.1177/0146167200267004.
Article
Google Scholar