Abstract
We examined how individuals may change their voices when speaking to attractive versus unattractive individuals, and if it were possible for others to perceive these vocal changes. In addition, we examined if any concurrent physiological effects occurred when speaking with individuals who varied in physical attractiveness. We found that both sexes used a lower-pitched voice and showed a higher level of physiological arousal when speaking to the more attractive, opposite-sex target. Furthermore, independent raters evaluated the voice samples directed toward the attractive target (versus the unattractive target) as sounding more pleasant when the two voice samples from the same person presented had a reasonably perceptually noticeable difference in pitch. These findings may have implications for the role voice plays in mate selection and attraction.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.


References
Amir, O., Biron-Shental, T., & Shabtai, E. (2006). Birth control pills and nonprofessional voice: Acoustic analyses. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 49(5), 1114–1126.
Anolli, L., & Ciceri, R. (2002). Analysis of the vocal profiles of male seduction: From exhibition to self-disclosure. Journal of General Psychology, 129, 149–169.
Apicella, C. L., Feinberg, D. R., & Marlowe, F. W. (2007). Voice pitch predicts reproductive success in male hunter-gatherers. Biology Letters, 3, 682–684.
Aron, A., Fisher, H., Mashek, D. J., Strong, G., Li, H., & Brown, L. L. (2005). Reward, motivation, and emotion systems associated with early-stage intense romantic love. Journal of Neurophysiology, 94, 327–337.
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2000). The neural basis of romantic love. NeuroReport, 11, 3829–3834.
Berry, D. (1990). Vocal attractiveness and vocal babyishness: Effects on stranger, self and friend impressions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 14, 141–153.
Collins, S. A. (2000). Men’s voices and women’s choices. Animal Behaviour, 60, 773–780.
Collins, S. A., & Missing, C. (2003). Vocal and visual attractiveness are related in women. Animal Behaviour, 65, 997–1004.
Daniel, H. J., & McCabe, R. B. (1992). Gender differences in the perception of vocal sexiness. In J. M. G. van der Dennen (Ed.), The nature of the sexes: The sociobiology of sex differences and the ‘‘battle of the sexes’’ (p. 55). The Netherlands: Origin Press.
Davitz, J. R. (1964). The communication of emotional meaning. New York: McGraw Hill.
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Scherer, K. (1976). Body movements and voice pitch in deceptive interaction. Semiotica, 16, 23–27.
Ethofer, T., Wiethoff, S., Anders, S., Kreifelts, B., Grod, W., & Wildgruber, D. (2007). The voices of seduction: Cross-gender effects in the processing of erotic prosody. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 334–337.
Evans, S., Neave, N., & Wakelin, D. (2006). Relationships between vocal characteristics and body size and shape in human males: An evolutionary explanation for a deep male voice. Biological Psychology, 72, 160–163.
Feinberg, D. R., DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., & Perrett, D. I. (2008). The role of femininity and averageness of voice pitch in aesthetic judgments of women’s voices. Perception, 37, 615–623.
Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2005). Manipulation of fundamental and formant frequencies influence the attractiveness of human male voices. Animal Behavior, 69, 561–568.
Hall, J. A., & Braunwald, K. G. (1981). Gender cues in conversations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 99–110.
Hess, E. H., & Polt, J. M. (1960). Pupil size as related to interest value of visual stimuli. Science, 132, 349–350.
Hughes, S. M., Dispenza, F., & Gallup, G. G., Jr. (2004). Ratings of voice attractiveness predict sexual behavior and body configuration. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 295–304.
Hughes, S. M., Harrison, M. A., & Gallup, G. G., Jr. (2002). The sound of symmetry: Voice as a marker of developmental instability. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 173–180.
Istvan, J., Griffitt, W., & Weidner, G. (1982). Sexual arousal and the polarization of perceived sexual attractiveness. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 4, 307–318.
Jones, B. C., Feinberg, D. R., DeBruine, L. M., Little, A. C., & Vukovic, J. (2008). Integrating cues of social interest and pitch in men’s preferences for women’s voices. Biology Letters, 4, 192–194.
Karpf, A. (2006). The human voice. New York, NY: Bloomsbury Publishing.
Kimble, C. E., & Seidel, S. D. (1991). Vocal signs of confidence. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 15, 99–105.
Kramer, E. R. (1963). Judgment of personal characteristics and emotions from nonverbal properties of speech. Psychological Bulletin, 60, 408–420.
Kramer, E. R. (1964). Elimination of cues in judgments of emotion from voice. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 390–396.
Ladefoged, P. (1996). Elements of acoustic phonetics (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Leaderbrand, K., Dekam, J., Morey, A., & Tuma, L (2008). The effects of voice pitch on perceptions of attractiveness: Do you sound hot or not. Winona State University Psychology Student Journal.
Montepare, J. M., & Vega, C. (1988). Women’s vocal reactions to intimate and casual male friends. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 103–113.
Oguchi, T., & Kikuchi, H. (1997). Voice and interpersonal attraction. Japanese Psychological Research, 39, 56–61.
Oksenberg, L., Coleman, L., & Cannell, C. F. (1986). Interviewers’ voice and refusal rates in telephone surveys. Public Opinion Quarterly, 50, 97–111.
Pemberton, C., McCormack, P., & Russell, A. (1998). Have women’s voices lowered across time? A cross-sectional study of Australian women’s voices. Journal of Voice, 12, 208–213.
Pipitone, R. N., & Gallup, G. G., Jr. (2008). Women’s attractiveness varies across the menstrual cycle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 268–274.
Puts, D. A. (2005). Mating context and menstrual phase affect women’s preferences for male voice pitch. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 388–397.
Puts, D. A., Gaulin, J. C., & Verdolini, K. (2006). Dominance of sexual dimorphism in human voice pitch. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 283–296.
Riding, D., Lonsdale, D., & Brown, B. (2006). The effects of average fundamental frequency and variance of fundamental frequency on male vocal attractiveness to women. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 30, 55–61.
Rousey, C. L., & Moriarty, A. E. (1965). Diagnostic implications of speech sounds: The reflections of developmental conflict and trauma. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.
Scherer, K. R. (1986). Vocal affect expression: A review and a model for future research. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 143–165.
Snyder, M., Tanke, E. D., & Berscheid, E. (1977). Social perception and interpersonal behavior: On the self-fulfilling nature of Social stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 656–666.
Steckler, N. A., & Rosenthal, R. (1985). Sex differences in nonverbal and verbal communication with bosses, peers, and subordinates. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70, 157–163.
Stern, R. M., Botto, R. W., & Herrick, C. D. (1972). Behavioral and physiological effects of false heart rate feedback: A replication and extension. Psychophysiology, 9, 21–29.
Streeter, L. A., Krauss, R. M., Geller, V. J., Olson, C. T., & Apple, W. (1977). Pitch changes during attempted deception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 345–350.
Taylor, S. F., Liberzon, I., & Koeppe, R. A. (2000). The effect of graded aversive stimuli on limbic and visual activation. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1415–1425.
Tuomi, S. K., & Fisher, J. E. (1979). Characteristics of a simulated sexy voice. Folia Phoniatrica, 31, 242–249.
Uchiyama, I., Hanari, T., Ito, T., & Takahashi, K. (1990). Patterns of psychological and physiological responses for common affects elicited by films. Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient, 33, 36–41.
Valins, S. (1967). Emotionality and information concerning internal reactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 458–463.
Van Bezooijen, R. (1995). Sociocultural aspects of pitch differences between Japanese and Dutch women. Language and Speech, 38, 253–265.
Vukovic, J., Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Welling, L. L. M., Little, A. C., et al. (2008). Self-rated attractiveness predicts individual differences in women’s preferences for masculine male voices. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 451–456.
Winton, W. N., Putnam, L. E., & Krauss, R. M. (1984). Facial and autonomic manifestations of the dimensional structure of emotion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 195–216.
Woll, S. B., & McFall, M. E. (1979). The effects of false feedback on attributed arousal and rated attractiveness in female subjects. Journal of Personality, 47, 214–229.
Zuckerman, M., & Driver, R. (1989). What sounds beautiful is good: The vocal attractiveness stereotype. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13, 67–82.
Acknowledgments
We wish to thank David Osgood and The Summer Albright College Research Experience Program (ACRE) for their support of this research, Rodney Warfield for his assistance in obtaining participants for this study, and Andrea Chapdelaine for helpful comments with analysis.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Hughes, S.M., Farley, S.D. & Rhodes, B.C. Vocal and Physiological Changes in Response to the Physical Attractiveness of Conversational Partners. J Nonverbal Behav 34, 155–167 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-010-0087-9
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-010-0087-9
Keywords
- Vocal changes
- Attractiveness
- Voice
- Physiological responses
- Romantic attraction