Abstract
Early literature found that holding more anti-Semitic attitudes positively predicted ability to discern whether a photograph was of a Jewish or non-Jewish person. This contradicts the well established finding that interpersonal sensitivity is generally associated with healthy psychological characteristics. In five new, previously unpublished studies we found that this relation was negative, such that more prejudiced individuals were now less accurate than less prejudiced individuals at a similar task, consistent with the general finding. A meta-analysis of all the studies showed that time was a significant moderator of the relation. Possible reasons for the temporal change are discussed.
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
While all of the new studies specifically measured anti-Semitic attitudes, some of the earlier studies utilized prejudice measures that combined anti-Semitism with other prejudicial attitudes. Allport and Kramer (1946), Elliott and Wittenberg (1955), and Lindzey and Rogolsky (1950) specifically looked at anti-Semitism while Himmelfarb (1966) and Dorfman et al. (1971) looked at a combination of prejudice toward Jews, African-Americans, and Catholics. Scodel and Austrin (1957) used the California F scale and Quanty et al. (1975) used the Allport-Vernon prejudice scale.
In order to save space in the text we provide the methodology only for the new studies. The original articles may be consulted to ascertain the slight differences in methodology among the original studies.
The formula for A′ was taken from Quanty et al. (1975): \( A^{\prime} = .5 + \frac{\left( {\text {Hits}} - {\text {False}}\;{\text {Alarms}} \right)\left( 1 + {\text {Hits}} - {\text {False}}\;{\text {Alarms}} \right)}{4{\text {Hits}}\left( 1 - {\text {False}}\;{\text {Alarms}} \right)} \)
Analyses to detect curvilinear trends were conducted in all five studies, but there was never any such evidence. Also, although in general across the five studies, the relation between accuracy and prejudice was more negative for women than for men, this difference was not statistically significant across all of the studies.
It is not unheard of for the personal characteristics associated with social attitudes to change over time. As Frank (2004) showed in his book What’s the Matter With Kansas?, liberal political values used to prevail among the working class while conservatism was the creed of the upper class, but in very recent history this association has reversed itself. It is possible that there has been a corresponding shift in the psychological characteristics associated with prejudice.
Since the submission of this article the first author has continued to explore the relation between prejudice towards different outgroups and other types of interpersonal sensitivity tasks (e.g., judging emotion). This research found a similar relationship between accuracy at judging emotion and a decoding deficit for highly prejudiced persons. The similar relation of both types of accuracy with prejudice suggests that prejudiced attitudes are negatively related to several different types of nonverbal decoding skills and not just one specific type of interpersonal sensitivity.
References
Allport, G. W., & Kramer, B. M. (1946). Some roots of prejudice. Journal of Psychology, 22, 9–39.
Andrzejewski, S. A. (2008). Interpersonal sensitivity and prejudice: A meta-analysis (manuscript submitted for publication).
Beal, D. J., O’Neal, E. C., Ong, J., & Ruscher, J. B. (2000). The ways and means of interracial aggression: Modern racists’ use of covert retaliation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1225–1238.
Bernieri, F. J. (2001). Toward a taxonomy of interpersonal sensitivity. In J. A. Hall & F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and measurement (pp. 3–20). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bernieri, F. J., Gillis, J. S., Davis, J. M., & Grahe, J. E. (1996). Dyad rapport and the accuracy of its judgment across situations: A lens model analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 110–129.
Borman, W. C. (1979). Individual differences correlates of accuracy in evaluating others’ performance effectiveness. Journal of Applied Psychological Measurement, 3, 103–115.
Bushman, K. W., & Bonacci, A. M. (2004). You’ve got mail: Using e-mail to examine the effect of prejudiced attitudes on discrimination against Arabs. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 753–759.
Carter, J. D., & Hall, J. A. (2008). Individual differences in the accuracy of detecting social covariations: Ecological sensitivity. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 439–455.
Carter, J. D., Hall, J. A., Carney, D. R., & Rosip, J. C. (2006). Individual differences in the acceptance of stereotyping. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 1103–1118.
Carter, L. F. (1948). The identification of “racial” membership. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 43, 279–286.
Christiansen, N. D., Wolcott-Burnam, S., Janovics, J. E., Burns, G. N., & Quirk, S. W. (2005). The good judge revisited: Individual differences in the accuracy of personality judgments. Journal of Human Performance, 18, 123–149.
Cline, V. B. (1955). Ability to judge personality assessed with a stress interview and sound-film technique. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 50, 183–187.
Devine, P. G., Monteith, M. J., Zuwerink, J. R., & Elliot, A. J. (1991). Prejudice with and without compunction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 817–830.
Dorfman, D. D., Keeve, S., & Saslow, C. (1971). Ethnic identification: A signal detection analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 373–379.
Dovidio, J. F. (2001). On the nature of contemporary prejudice: The third wave. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 829–849.
Elliott, D. N., & Wittenberg, B. H. (1955). Accuracy of identification of Jewish and non-Jewish photographs. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 57, 339–341.
Frank, T. (2004). What’s the matter with Kansas? How conservatives won the heart of America. New York: Holt.
Funder, D. C., & Harris, M. J. (1986). On the several facets of personality assessment: The case of social acuity. Journal of Personality, 54, 528–550.
Gangestad, S. W., & Snyder, M. (2000). Self-monitoring: Appraisal and reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 530–555.
Gifford, R. (1994). A lens-mapping framework for understanding the encoding and decoding of interpersonal dispositions in nonverbal behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 398–412.
Grier, J. B. (1971). Nonparametric indices for sensitivity and bias: Computing formulas. Psychological Bulletin, 75, 424–429.
Hall, J. A., & Andrzejewski, S. A. (2008). Who draws accurate first impressions? Personal correlates of sensitivity to nonverbal cues. In J. J. Skowronski & N. Ambady (Eds.), First impressions. New York: Guilford.
Hall, J. A., Andrzejewski, S. A., Yopchick, J. E. (2008). Psychosocial correlates of interpersonal sensitivity: A meta-analysis. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior (in press).
Hall, J. A., & Carter, J. D. (1999). Gender-stereotype accuracy as an individual difference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 350–359.
Hightower, E. (1997). Psychosocial characteristics of subtle and blatant racists as compared to tolerant individuals. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 53, 369–374.
Himmelfarb, S. (1966). Studies in the perception of ethnic group members: Accuracy, response bias, and anti-Semitism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 347–355.
Karabel, J. (2005). The chosen: The hidden history of admission and exclusion at Harvard, Yale, and Princeton. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Kawakami, K., Dion, K. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1998). Racial prejudice and stereotype activation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 407–416.
Lindzey, G., & Rogolsky, S. (1950). Prejudice and identification of minority group membership. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 45, 37–53.
Lund, F. H., & Berg, W. C. (1946). Identifiability of nationality characteristics. Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 77–83.
Matsumoto, D., LeRoux, J., Wilson-Cohn, C., Raroque, J., Kooken, K., Ekman, P., et al. (2000). A new test to measure emotion recognition ability: Matsumoto and Ekman’s Japanese and Caucasian Brief Affect Recognition Test (JACBART). Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 179–209.
Murphy, N. A., Hall, J. A., & Colvin, C. R. (2003). Accurate intelligence assessments in social interactions: Mediators and gender effects. Journal of Personality, 71, 465–493.
Nowicki, S., Jr., & Duke, M. (1994). Individual differences in the nonverbal communication of affect: The Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy Scale. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 9–35.
Pettigrew, T. F. (1981). The mental health impact. In B. P. Bowser & R. G. Hunt (Eds.), Impacts of racism on white Americans (pp. 97–118). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Pulos, L., & Spilka, B. (1961). Perceptual selectivity, memory, and anti-Semitism. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62, 690–692.
Quanty, M. B., Keats, J. A., & Harkins, S. G. (1975). Prejudice and criteria for identification of ethnic photographs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 449–454.
Realo, A., Allik, J., Nõlvak, A., Valk, R., Ruus, T., Schmidt, M., et al. (2003). Mind-reading ability: Beliefs and performance. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 420–445.
Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research (rev. ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Rosenthal, R., Hall, J. A., DiMatteo, M. R., Rogers, P. L., & Archer, D. (1979). Sensitivity to nonverbal communication: The PONS test. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Scodel, A., & Austrin, H. (1957). The perception of Jewish photographs by non-Jews and Jews. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 54, 278–280.
Smith, T. W. (1990). GSS Topical Report No. 19: Ethnic images. Chicago, IL: National Opinion Research Center.
Sprinthall, R. C., Lambert, M., & Sturm, M. (1971). Anti-Semitism: Some perceptual correlates among Jews and non-Jews. The Journal of Social Psychology, 84, 57–63.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding authors
Additional information
The authors thank Fred Gordon for his theoretical insights, Paul White for his helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript, and Lisa Babin, Christine Barry, Vanessa Castro, Michelle Cowdrick, Karima Sharif, Katie Sobiech, and Halle Thurnauer for assistance in conducting the studies.
Appendix
Appendix
Examples of criteria for determining group membership for identification tasks
Group | Clue(s) |
---|---|
Jewish | Debbie Friedman, singer of Jewish music Tovah Feldshuh, portrays Golda Meir on stage Benny Levy, Jewish philosopher Jonathan Nissim Schulhof, married by rabbi Eugene Eisenberg, funeral at Jewish funeral home Benjamin Brafman, orthodox Jew Benjamin Blech, rabbi |
Anglo non-Jewish | Colleen Meade, married by Catholic priest Donald Menzies, funeral in Catholic church Denis Hurley, Catholic priest Mark Lanier, talks about his Christmas celebration and going to church Timothy Lucie, belongs to evangelical church Robbie O’Connell, singer of Irish songs |
Hispanic | Eunice Zavala, wife of illegal Mexican immigrant Juan Escalante, father emigrated from Mexico Marivel Gutierez, works in San Antonio, reference to Mexico Thelma Delgado-Josey, Spanish bilingual teacher Felix Arroyo, described as Latino Chita Rivera, Hispanic actress |
Arab | Djillali Antar, parents are from Algeria Ihad Rashad, comes from Egypt Abdul Raudi, comes from Morocco Saalih Allie, married by Islamic cleric Irshad Manji, says she is Muslim Tanisha Fazal, friend of the family gave Islamic blessing at ceremony |
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Andrzejewski, S.A., Hall, J.A. & Salib, E.R. Anti-Semitism and Identification of Jewish Group Membership from Photographs. J Nonverbal Behav 33, 47–58 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-008-0060-z
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-008-0060-z