Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Lay Theories for Life Satisfaction and the Belief that Life Gets Better and Better

  • Research Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Happiness Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Many individuals believe that life gets better and better over time. To examine the sources and significance of such beliefs, we examined lay theories for life satisfaction (LS) in relation to individuals’ beliefs concerning how their LS was unfolding over time. Two studies were conducted with online participants: one correlational (Study 1; N = 320, M age = 30.39, 55% male), the other experimental (Study 2; N = 321, M age = 30.46, 53% male). In both studies more incremental (vs. entity) lay theories were associated with more steeply inclining subjective trajectories for LS. Furthermore, both sets of beliefs had unique effects on individuals’ goal-striving toward a brighter future life, as well as psychological adjustment (self-efficacy, hope, optimism, positive affect, negative affect). Thus, lay theories and subjective trajectories for LS share a common assumption concerning change in life satisfaction over time. And each set of beliefs plays a unique role in positive functioning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baltes, P. (1997). On the incomplete architecture of human ontogeny: Selection, optimization, and compensation as foundation of developmental theory. American Psychologist, 52, 366–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P. B., & Smith, J. (2003). New frontiers in the future of aging: From successful aging of the young old to the dilemmas of the Fourth Age. Gerontology, 49, 123–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumgardner, S. R., & Crothers, M. R. (2009). Positive psychology. Upper Sadle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2003). Life scripts to help maintain autobiographical memories of highly positive, but not highly negative events. Memory and Cognition, 31, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2004). Cultural life scripts structure recall from autobiographical memory. Memory & Cognition, 32, 427–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and intervention. Child Development, 78, 246–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boldero, J., & Francis, J. (2002). Goals, standards, and the self: Reference values serving different function. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 232–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bunda, K., & Busseri, M. A. (in press). Lay theories of health, self-rated health, and health-behavior intentions. Journal of Health Psychology.

  • Burnette, J. L., O’Boyle, E. H., VanEpps, E. M., Pollack, J. M., & Pinkel, E. J. (2013). Mind-sets matter: A meta-analytic review of implicit theories and self-regulation. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 655–701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Busseri, M. A. (2013). How dispositional optimists and pessimists evaluate their past, present and anticipated future life satisfaction: A lifespan approach. European Journal of Personality, 27, 185–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Busseri, M. A., & Choma, B. L. (2016). Re-evaluating the link between dispositional optimism and positive functioning using a temporally-expanded perspective. Journal of Positive Psychology, 11, 286–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Busseri, M. A., Choma, B. L., & Sadava, S. W. (2009a). Functional or fantasy? Examining the implications of subjective temporal perspective “trajectories” for life satisfaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 295–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Busseri, M. A., Choma, B. L., & Sadava, S. W. (2009b). “As good as it gets” or “The best is yet to come”? How optimists and pessimists view their past, present, and anticipated future life satisfaction. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 352–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Busseri, M. A., Choma, B. L., & Sadava, S. W. (2012). Subjective temporal trajectories for subjective well-being. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 7, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Busseri, M. A., Malinowski, A., & Choma, B. L. (2013). Are optimists oriented uniquely toward the future? Investigating dispositional optimism from a temporally-expanded perspective. Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 533–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Busseri, M. A., & Merrick, H. D. (2016). Subjective trajectories for life satisfaction: A self-discrepancy perspective. Motivation & Emotion, 40, 389–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Busseri, M. A., & Peck, E. (2015). Do (even) depressed individuals believe that life gets better and better? The link between depression and subjective trajectories for LS. Clinical Psychological Science, 3, 715–725.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Busseri, M. A., & Sadava, S. W. (2011). A review of the tripartite structure of subjective well-being: Implications for conceptualization, operationalization, and synthesis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15, 290–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cantril, H. (1965). Pattern of human concerns. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carstensen, L. L. (2006). The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science, 312, 1913–1915.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, G., Gully, S. M., & Eden, D. (2001). Validation of a new general self-efficacy scale. Organizational research methods, 4, 62–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiu, C.-Y., Hong, Y.-Y., & Dweck, C. S. (1997). Lay dispositionism and implicit theories of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 19–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Choma, B. L., Busseri, M. A., & Sadava, S. W. (2014). Deciphering subjective trajectories for life satisfaction using self-versus-normative other discrepancies, self-esteem, and hope. European Journal of Personality, 28, 107–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542–575.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E. (2008). Myths in the science of happiness, and directions for future research. In M. Eid, & R. J. Larsen (Eds.), The science of subjective well-being (pp. 493–514). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi, D.-W., Oishi, S., et al. (2010). New well-being measures: Short scales to assess flourishing and positive and negative feelings. Social Indicators Research, 97, 143–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Pennsylvania, USA: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (2014, November). The power of yet [Video file]. Retrieved from https://www.ted.com/talks/carol_dweck_the_power_of_believing_that_you_can_improve.

  • Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C-y, & Hong, Y-y. (1995). Implicit theories and their role in judgments and reactions: A world from two perspectives. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 267–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleeson, W., & Heckhausen, J. (1997). More or less “me” in the past, present, and future: Perceived lifetime personality during adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 12, 125–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fujita, F., & Diener, E. (2005). Life satisfaction set point: Stability and change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, M. W., Lopez, S. J., & Pressman, S. D. (2013). Optimism is universal: Exploring the presence and benefits of optimism in a representative sample of the world. Journal of Personality, 81, 429–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J., Dixon, R. A., & Baltes, P. B. (1989). Gains and losses in development throughout adulthood as perceived by different adult age groups. Developmental Psychology, 25, 109–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hong, Y-Y, Chiu, C-Y, Dweck, C. S., Lin, D. M.-S., & Wan, W. (1999). Implicit theories, attributions, and coping: A meaning system approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 588–599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howe, L. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2015). Changes in self-definition impede recovery from rejection. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42, 54–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howell, A. J. (2017). Believing in change: Reviewing the role of implicit theories in psychological dysfunction. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 36, 437–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howell, A. J., Passmore, H.-A., & Holder, M. D. (2016). Implicit theories of well-being predict well-being and the endorsement of therapeutic lifestyle changes. Journal of Happiness Studies, 17, 237–2363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, J. S. (2015). Support for the domain specificity of implicit beliefs about persons, intelligence, and morality. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 195–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keyes, C. L., & Ryff, C. D. (2000). Subjective change in mental health: A self-concept theory. Social Psychological Quarterly, 63, 264–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kilpatrick, F. P., & Cantril, H. (1960). Self-anchoring scaling: A measure of individuals’ unique reality worlds. Journal of Individual Psychology, 16, 158–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lachman, M. E., Röcke, C., Rosnick, C., & Ryff, C. D. (2008). Realism and illusion in Americans’ temporal views of their life satisfaction age differences in reconstructing the past and anticipating the future. Psychological Science, 19, 889–897.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lazowski, R. A., & Hulleman, C. S. (2015). Motivation interventions in education: A meta-analytic review. Review of Educational Research, 86, 602–640.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R. (2007). Adaptation and the set-point model of subjective well-being: Does happiness change after major life events? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 75–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magaletta, P. R., & Oliver, J. M. (1999). The hope construct, will, and ways: Their relations with self-efficacy, optimism, and general well-being. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, 539–551.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, K. D., & McMullen, M. N. (2003). A reflection and evaluation model of comparative thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7, 244–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McIntosh, C. N. (2001). Report on the construct validity of the temporal satisfaction with life scale. Social Indicators Research, 54, 37–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molden, D. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Finding “meaning” in psychology. American Psychologist, 61, 192–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neel, R., & Lassetter, B. (2015). Growing fixed with age: Lay theories of malleability are target age-specific. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, 1505–1522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newby-Clark, I. R., & Ross, M. (1998). Conceiving the past and future. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 807–818.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, E., & Kardas, M. (2016). The implicit meaning of (my) change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111, 882.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oettingen, G., Kappes, H. B., Guttenberg, K. B., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2015). Self-regulation of time management: Mental contrasting with implementation intentions. European Journal of Social Psychology, 45, 218–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oettingen, G., Mayer, D., Sevincer, A. T., Stephens, E. J., Pak, H. J., & Hagenah, M. (2009). Mental contrasting and goal commitment: The mediating role of energization. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 608–622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oettingen, G., Mayer, D., Thorpe, J. S., Janetzke, H., & Lorenz, S. (2005). Turning fantasies about positive and negative futures into self-improvement goals. Motivation and Emotion, 29, 236–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pavot, W., Diener, E., & Suh, E. (1998). The temporal satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 70, 340–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peetz, J., & Wilson, A. (2008). The temporally extended self: The relation of past and future selves to current identity, motivation, and goal pursuit. Social and Personality Compass, 2, 2090–2106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaks, J. E., Levy, S. R., & Dweck, C. S. (2009). Lay theories of personality: Cornerstones of meaning in social cognition. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3, 1069–1081.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaks, J. E., & Stetcher, K. (2007). Unexpected improvement, decline, and stasis: A prediction confidence perspective on achievement success and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 667–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Röcke, C., & Lachman, M. E. (2008). Perceived trajectories of life satisfaction across past, present, and future: Profiles and correlates of subjective change in young, middle-aged, and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 23, 833.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryff, C. D. (1991). Possible selves in adulthood and old age: A tale of shifting horizons. Psychology and Aging, 6, 286–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): A re-evaluation of the life orientation test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1063–1078.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schroder, H. S., Dawood, S., Yalch, M. M., Donnellan, M. B., & Moser, J. S. (2016). Evaluating the domain-specificity of mental health-related mind-sets. Social Psychology and Personality Science, 7, 508–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shanahan, E., & Busseri, M. A. (2016). Life gets better and better: Cultural life script theory and subjective trajectories for life satisfaction. European Journal of Personality, 30, 564–579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shmotkin, D. (2005). Happiness in the face of adversity: Reformulating the dynamic and modular bases of subjective well-being. Review of General Psychology, 9, 291–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, C. R., Harris, C., Anderson, J. R., Holleran, S. A., Irving, L. M., Sigmon, S. T., et al. (1991). The will and the ways: Development and validation of an individual-differences measure of hope. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 570–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, C. R., Lopez, S. J., & Pedrotti, J. T. (2010). Positive psychology: The scientific and practical explorations of human strengths. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staudinger, U. M., Bluck, S., & Herzberg, P. Y. (2003). Looking back and looking ahead: Adult age differences in consistency of diachronous ratings of subjective well-being. Psychology and Aging, 18, 13–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steimer, A., & Mata, A. (2016). Motivated implicit theories of personality: My weaknesses will go away, but my strengths are here to stay. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42, 415–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tamir, M., John, O. Pl, Srivastava, S., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Implicit theories of emotion: Affective and social outcomes across a major life transition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 731–744.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., Neter, E. N., & Wayment, H. A. (1995). Self-evaluation processes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1278–1287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Tongeren, D. R., & Burnette, J. L. (2018). Do you believe happiness can change? An investigation of the relationship between happiness mindsets, well-being, and satisfaction. Journal of Positive Psychology, 13, 101–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, C. L. P., & Wilson, A. E. (2015). Implicit theories of change and stability moderate effects of subjective distance on the remembered self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, 1167–1179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yeager, D. S., Johnson, R., Spitzer, B. J., Trzesniewski, K. H., Powers, J., & Dweck, S. S. (2014). The far-reaching effects of believing people can change: Implicit theories of personality shape stress, health, and achievement during adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 867–884.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

Funding

This research was supported by an Insight Grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada to the first author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael A. Busseri.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Appendix

Appendix

1.1 Lay Theory for Life Satisfaction Scale

This questionnaire has been designed to investigate ideas about your life satisfaction. There are no right or wrong answers. We are interested in your ideas. Using the scale below, please indicate that extent to which you agree or disagree with each of the following statements by circling the number that corresponds to your opinion next to each statement.

 

Strongly agree

Agree

Mostly agree

Mostly disagree

Disagree

Strongly Disagree

1. I have a certain amount of life satisfaction and I can’t really do much to change it.

1

2

3

4

5

6

2. My life satisfaction is something that I can’t change very much.

1

2

3

4

5

6

3. No matter who you are, you can change your life satisfaction a lot.

1

2

3

4

5

6

4. To be honest, I can’t really change how satisfied I am with my life.

1

2

3

4

5

6

5. I can always substantially change how satisfied I am with my life.

1

2

3

4

5

6

6. I can experience new things, but I can’t really change my basic level of life satisfaction.

1

2

3

4

5

6

7. No matter how much life satisfaction I have, I can always change it quite a bit.

1

2

3

4

5

6

8. I can change even my basic level of life satisfaction considerably.

1

2

3

4

5

6

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Busseri, M.A., Samani, M.N. Lay Theories for Life Satisfaction and the Belief that Life Gets Better and Better. J Happiness Stud 20, 1647–1672 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-018-0016-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-018-0016-x

Keywords

Navigation