Skip to main content
Log in

Materialism, Spending, and Affect: An Event-Sampling Study of Marketplace Behavior and Its Affective Costs

  • Research Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Happiness Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research on materialism has burgeoned in the last two decades, yet little is known about how people higher versus lower in this consumer values orientation differ in their day-to-day spending habits and in their emotional reactions to spending on purchases. The present study used an event-sampling method over a 3-week period to address these questions in a community adult sample. Results showed that over the course of the sampling period, high materialists made more discretionary purchases and spent more money on necessity purchases than did those lower in materialism, even though their incomes did not differ. Despite higher levels of spending, high materialists experienced a “letdown” after spending, as they reported more post-purchase unpleasant affect than did low materialists. This result was not moderated by level of dispositional unpleasant affect, purchase type, or purchase amounts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The criterion that individuals be working in the daytime helped to ensure that there was homogeneity in diurnal activity patterns across participants.

  2. In these and all of the following analyses involving income, similarly significant results were found using natural log-transformed personal and household income. Analyses were also conducted using household income weighted by number of persons in the household; this variable was generally more weakly related to the dependent variables of interest than were the other income variables noted here.

  3. The fit of a cosine function was also examined, but a sine function provided a better (though still non-significant) fit. We tested for septurnal, or 7-day weekly cyclicity because a number of social and personality phenomena have been found to follow a weekly cycle (e.g., Brown 2000; Brown and Moskowitz 1998; Larsen and Kasimatis 1991; Reis et al. 2000). In the realm of spending, we expected that full-time working people would tend to have more spending occasions in the time period leading up to and during the weekend than during the majority of the work week. Cyclicity is most commonly tested using either a dummy variable approach or the trigonometric approach used here (Bowerman and O’Connell 1993). Because we were not interested in specific day-of-week effects, we chose the latter approach, which allows for fewer terms in model equations.

  4. A continuous time variable was created, in which the day and time that each record form was completed is used to create a continuous time variable which starts at day 1, record 1, and runs linearly upward to day n, record n. For each sampling record, the number of minutes after the pager signal that the form was completed was subtracted from the actual time of record completion to derive the actual time to which each record's data refers. Schwartz and Stone (1998) discuss the incorporation of time into SAS PROC MIXED to test for autocorrelation.

  5. We also examined the possible moderating role of frequency of spending over the 3-week period on the relation between materialism and post-spending unpleasant affect. Using data aggregated over the event-sampling phase of the study, multiple regression analyses on necessity and discretionary spending separately found that frequency of spending did not moderate the effect of materialism (or show a main effect) on spending-related unpleasant affect.

References

  • Belk, R. W. (1985). Materialism: Trait aspects living in the material world. Journal of Consumer Research, 12, 265–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, B. L., & O’Connell, R. T. (1993). Forecasting and time-series (3rd ed.). Belmont, CA: Duxbury.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, K. W. (2000). Emotional body, physical mind: An exploration of the psychosomatic system through the lens of day-to-day experience. Doctoral dissertation, ProQuest Information & Learning.

  • Brown, K. W., & Moskowitz, D. S. (1998). Dynamic stability of behavior: The rhythms of our interpersonal lives. Journal of Personality, 66, 105–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods. Newbury Park, NJ: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bureau of Economic Analysis (2001). Personal income and outlays. Retrieved August 17, 2004 from http://www.bea.doc.gov/bea/regional/spi/drill.cfm

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 526–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2002). Will money increase subjective well-being? Social Indicators Research, 57, 119–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1984). The independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1105–1117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Suh, E. M., & Lucas, R. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Tay, L., & Oishi, S. (2013). Rising income and the subjective well-being of nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(2), 267–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dittmar, H., Bond, R., Hurst, M., & Kasser, T. (2014). The relationship between materialism and personal well-being: A meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107, 879–924.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donnelly, G., Iyer, R., & Howell, R. T. (2012). The Big Five personality traits, material values, and financial well-being of self-described money managers. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(6), 1129–1142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duriez, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Soenens, B., & De Witte, H. (2007). The social costs of extrinsic relative to intrinsic goal pursuits: Their relation with social dominance and racial and ethnic prejudice. Journal of Personality, 75(4), 757–782.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fournier, S., & Guiry, M. (1993). “An emerald green jaguar, a house on Nantucket, and an African safari”. Wish lists and consumption dreams in materialist society. Advances in Consumer Research, 20, 352–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B. S., & Stutzer, A. (2002). What can economists learn from happiness research? Journal of Economic Literature, 40, 402–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Furnham, A., & Argyle, M. (1998). The psychology of money. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garðarsdóttir, R. B., & Dittmar, H. (2012). The relationship of materialism to debt and financial well-being: The case of Iceland’s perceived prosperity. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(3), 471–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, M. E., Gorn, G. J., Peracchio, L. A., & Bamossy, G. (2003). Understanding materialism among youth. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13, 278–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hudders, L., & Pandelaere, M. (2012). The silver lining of materialism: The impact of luxury consumption on subjective well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 13(3), 411–437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hurst, M., Dittmar, H., Bond, R., & Kasser, T. (2013). The relationship between materialistic values and environmental attitudes and behaviors: A meta-analysis. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 36, 257–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jiang, J., Song, Y., Ke, Y., Wang, R., & Liu, H. (in press). Is disciplinary culture a moderator between materialism and subjective well-being? A three-wave longitudinal study. Journal of Happiness Studies. doi:10.1007/s10902-015-9649-1

  • Kasser, T. (2002). The high price of materialism. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T. (2005). Frugality, generosity, and materialism in children and adolescents. In K. A. Moore & L. H. Lippman (Eds.), What do children need to flourish? (pp. 357–373). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T. (in press). Materialistic values and goals. Annual Review of Psychology.

  • Kasser, T., & Ahuvia, A. (2002). Materialistic values and well-being in business students. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 137–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 280–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kreft, I., & deLeeuw, J. (1998). Introducing multilevel modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, R. J., & Kasimatis, M. (1991). Day-to-day physical symptoms: Individual differences in the occurrence, duration, and emotional concomitants of minor daily illnesses. Journal of Personality, 59, 387–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lebergott, S. (1993). Pursuing happiness: American consumers in the twentieth century. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Machleit, K. A., & Eroglu, S. A. (2000). Describing and measuring emotional response to shopping experience. Journal of Business Research, 49, 101–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mankiw, N. G. (2001). Principles of macroeconomics (2nd ed.). Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt College Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • McHoskey, J. W. (1999). Machiavellianism, intrinsic versus extrinsic goals, and social interest: A self-determination theory analysis. Motivation and Emotion, 23(4), 267–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nepomuceno, M. V., & Laroche, M. (2015). The impact of materialism and anti-consumption lifestyles on personal debt and account balances. Journal of Business Research, 68, 654–664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niemiec, C. P., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2009). The path taken: Consequences of attaining intrinsic and extrinsic aspirations in post-college life. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 291–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oropesa, R. S. (1995). Consumer possessions, consumer passions, and subjective well-being. Sociological Forum, 10, 215–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Otero-López, J. M., & Villardefrancos, E. (2013). Five-factor model personality traits, materialism, and excessive buying: A mediational analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 767–772.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Otero-López, J. M., & Villardefrancos, E. (2015). Compulsive buying and life aspirations: An analysis of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. Personality and Individual Differences, 76, 166–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reis, H. T., Sheldon, K. M., Gable, S. L., Roscoe, J., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). Daily well-being: The role of autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 419–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richins, M. L. (2013). When wanting is better than having: Materialism, transformation expectations, and product-evoked emotions in the purchase process. Journal of Consumer Research, 40, 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richins, M. L., & Dawson, S. (1992). A consumer values orientation for materialism and its measurement: Scale development and validation. Journal of Consumer Research, 19, 303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richins, M. L., & Rudmin, F. W. (1994). Materialism and economic psychology. Journal of Economic Psychology, 15, 217–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SAS Institute. (1992). SAS technical report P-229, SAS/STAT software: Changes and enhancements. Cary, NC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • SAS Institute. (1997). SAS/STAT software: Changes and enhancements through release 6.12. Cary, NC: Author.

  • Schor, J. (1998). The overspent American: Upscaling, downshifting, and the new consumer. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, J. E., & Stone, A. A. (1998). Strategies for analyzing ecological momentary assessment data. Health Psychology, 17, 6–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scitovsky, T. (1992). The joyless economy: The psychology of human satisfaction. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., Gunz, A., Nichols, C. P., & Ferguson, Y. (2010). Extrinsic value orientation and affective forecasting: Overestimating the rewards, underestimating the costs. Journal of Personality, 78, 149–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (1998). Pursuing personal goals: Skills enable progress, but not all progress is beneficial. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1319–1331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., Sheldon, M. S., & Osbaldiston, R. (2000). Prosocial values and group assortation. Human Nature, 11(4), 387–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsang, J. A., Carpenter, T. P., Roberts, J. A., Frisch, M. B., & Carlisle, R. D. (2014). Why are materialists less happy? The role of gratitude and need satisfaction in the relationship between materialism and life satisfaction. Personality and Individual Differences, 64, 62–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • U.S. Census Bureau (2001). Historical income tables—households. Retrieved August 17, 2004 from http://www.census.gov/hhes/income/histinc/h09.html

  • Vansteenkiste, M., Duriez, B., Simons, J., & Soenens, B. (2006). Materialistic values and well-being among business students: Further evidence for their detrimental effect. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36, 2892–2908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. J. (2003). The relationship of materialism to spending tendencies, saving, and debt. Journal of Economic Psychology, 24, 723–739.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • West, S. G., & Hepworth, J. T. (1991). Statistical issues in the study of temporal data: Daily experiences. Journal of Personality, 59, 611–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective forecasting. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 345–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kirk Warren Brown.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Brown, K.W., Kasser, T., Ryan, R.M. et al. Materialism, Spending, and Affect: An Event-Sampling Study of Marketplace Behavior and Its Affective Costs. J Happiness Stud 17, 2277–2292 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-015-9694-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-015-9694-9

Keywords

Navigation