Advertisement

Journal of Happiness Studies

, Volume 17, Issue 4, pp 1741–1752 | Cite as

Why Negative Feelings are Important when Assessing Well-Being

  • Bjørn GrindeEmail author
Review Article

Abstract

Present knowledge on the human brain, and the evolutionary process that shaped it, allow for a biological model for what happiness is about. I shall discuss the assessment of happiness; arguing that based on this model, the focus should be on estimating negative affect. Feelings are a feature introduced by evolution for the purpose of evaluating behavioral options. They tend to be either positive or negative due to their role in directing behavior either toward opportunities or away from danger. Happiness can be construed as the net sum of positive and negative feelings. A salient point is that the evidence suggests the mammalian brain has a positive mood as a default setting, implying that people are reasonably happy as long as punishing circuits are inactive. Activation of negative feelings—particularly in the form of anxiety, depression and pain—is therefore likely to be the main cause of a substandard level of well-being. Consequently, instruments that probe negative affect may offer a reasonable strategy, or at least important additional information, when evaluating happiness. These instruments may also provide a suitable foundation for policy making.

Keywords

Measuring well-being Mood circuitry Default contentment Indexes Brain reward Brain punishment 

References

  1. Berridge, K. C. (2003). Pleasures of the brain. Brain and Cognition, 52(1), 106–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Blood, A. J., & Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings of National Academy of Science USA, 98(20), 11818–11823.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bloom, D. E., Cafiero, E., Jané-Llopis, E., Abrahams-Gessel, S., Bloom, L. R., Fathima, S., et al. (2011). The global economic burden of noncommunicable diseases. New York: World Economic Forum.Google Scholar
  4. Breivik, H., Collett, B., Ventafridda, V., Cohen, R., & Gallacher, D. (2006). Survey of chronic pain in Europe: prevalence, impact on daily life, and treatment. European Journal of Pain, 10(4), 287–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Cabanac, M. (1992). Pleasure: the common currency. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 155(2), 173–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Chase, D. L., & Koelle, M. R. (2007). Biogenic amine neurotransmitters in C. elegans. WormBook. doi: 10.1895/wormbook.1.132.1.Google Scholar
  7. Cox, J. J., Reimann, F., Nicholas, A. K., Thornton, G., Roberts, E., Springell, K., et al. (2006). An SCN9A channelopathy causes congenital inability to experience pain. Nature, 444(7121), 894–898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cummins, R. A., Ning, L., Wooden, M., & Stokes, M. (2014). A demonstration of set-points for subjective wellbeing. Journal of Happiness Studies, 15, 183–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cummins, R. A., & Wooden, M. (2014). Personal resilience in times of crisis: The implications of SWB homeostasis and set-points. Journal of Happiness Studies, 15, 223–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Hedonia, eudaimonia, and well-being: an introduction. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9(1), 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Diener, E., & Diener, C. (1996). Most people are happy. Psychological Science, 7(3), 181–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1984). The independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(5), 1105–1117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction With Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Diener, E., Lucas, R., Schimmack, U., & Helltwell, J. (2009). Well-being for public policy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Diener, E., Ng, W., Harter, J., & Arora, R. (2010a). Wealth and happiness across the world: material prosperity predicts life evaluation, whereas psychosocial prosperity predicts positive feeling. Journal of Personality and Soccal Psychology, 99(1), 52–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi, D. W., Oishi, S., et al. (2010b). New Well-being Measures: Short Scales to Assess Flourishing and Positive and Negative Feelings. Social Indicators Research, 97(2), 143–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302(5643), 290–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Feinstein, J. S. (2013). Lesion studies of human emotion and feeling. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 23(3), 304–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Fredrickson, B. L., Grewen, K. M., Coffey, K. A., Algoe, S. B., Firestine, A. M., Arevalo, J. M. G., et al. (2013). A functional genomic perspective on human well-being. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 110(33), 13684–13689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Grinde, B. (2002). Happiness in the perspective of evolutionary psychology. Journal of Happiness Studies, 3, 331–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Grinde, B. (2005). An approach to the prevention of anxiety-related disorders based on evolutionary medicine. Preventive Medicine, 40, 904–909.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Grinde, B. (2009). Can the concept of discords help us find the causes of mental diseases? Medical Hypotheses, 73(1), 106–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Grinde, B. (2012a). The biology of happiness. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Grinde, B. (2012b). Darwinian happiness—Evolution as a guide for living and understanding human behavior (2nd ed.). Princeton: The Darwin Press.Google Scholar
  25. Grinde, B. (2013). The evolutionary rationale for consciousness. Biological Theory, 7, 227–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Headey, B., & Wearing, A. (1988). The sense of relative superiority - Central to well-being. Social Indicators Research, 20(5), 497–516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Helliwell, J. F., Layard, R., & Sachs, J. (2012). World happiness report. New York: United Nations.Google Scholar
  28. Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hoorens, V. (1995). Self-favoring biases, self-presentation, and the self-other asymmetry in social-comparison. Journal of Personality, 63(4), 793–817.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Huppert, F. A., Baylis, N., & Keverne, B. (Eds.). (2005). The science of well-being. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Izuma, K., Saito, D. N., & Sadato, N. (2010). Processing of the Incentive for Social Approval in the Ventral Striatum during Charitable Donation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(4), 621–631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kahneman, D., & Deaton, A. (2010). High income improves evaluation of life but not emotional well-being. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107(38), 16489–16493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kahneman, D., & Krueger, A. B. (2006). Developments in the measurement of subjective well-being. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 20(1), 3–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Knight, R. G., Chisholm, B. J., Paulin, J. M., & Waal-Manning, H. J. (1988). The Spielberger Anger Expression Scale: some psychometric data. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 27, 279–281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Koeter, M. W. (1992). Validity of the GHQ and SCL anxiety and depression scales: a comparative study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 24(4), 271–279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Kringelbach, M. L., & Berridge, K. C. (2009). Towards a functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness. Trends in Cognitive Science, 13(11), 479–487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Leknes, S., & Tracey, I. (2008). A common neurobiology for pain and pleasure. Nature Reviews Neurosciences, 9(4), 314–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Lieberman, M. D., & Eisenberger, N. I. (2009). Neuroscience pains and pleasures of social life. Science, 323(5916), 890–891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Lykken, D. (2000). Happiness: The nature and nurture of joy and contentment. New York: St. Martin’s Griffin.Google Scholar
  40. Lyubomirsky, S., & Lepper, H. S. (1999). A measure of subjective happiness: Preliminary reliability and construct validation. Social Indicators Research, 46(2), 137–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Mitchell, R. L. C., & Phillips, L. H. (2007). The psychological, neurochemical and functional neuroanatomical mediators of the effects of positive and negative mood on executive functions. Neuropsychologia, 45(4), 617–629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., Kokaua, J., Milne, B. J., Polanczyk, G., et al. (2010). How common are common mental disorders? Evidence that lifetime prevalence rates are doubled by prospective versus retrospective ascertainment. Psychological Medicine, 40(6), 899–909.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Nieto-Fernandez, F., Andrieux, S., Idrees, S., Bagnall, C., Pryor, S. C., & Sood, R. (2009). The effect of opioids and their antagonists on the nocifensive response of Caenorhabditis elegans to noxious thermal stimuli. Invertebrate Neuroscience, 9(3–4), 195–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. O’Connor, M. F., Wellisch, D. K., Stanton, A. L., Eisenberger, N. I., Irwin, M. R., & Lieberman, M. D. (2008). Craving love? Enduring grief activates brain’s reward center. Neuroimage, 42(2), 969–972.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Oishi, S. (2010). Culture and well-being: Conceptual and methodological issues. In E. Diener, J. F. Helliwell, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), International differences in well-being (pp. 34–69). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neruoscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  47. Pavot, W. (2008). The assessment of subjective well-being: Successes and shortfalls. In M. Eid & R. Larsen (Eds.), The science of subjective well-being (pp. 124–140). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  48. Schimmack, U. (2008). The structure of subjective well-being. In M. Eid & R. J. Larsen (Eds.), The science of subjective well-being (pp. 97–123). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  49. Stice, E., Figlewicz, D. P., Gosnell, B. A., Levine, A. S., & Pratt, W. E. (2013). The contribution of brain reward circuits to the obesity epidemic. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(9), 2047–2058.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Tabibnia, G., & Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Fairness and cooperation are rewarding - Evidence from social cognitive neuroscience. Social Cognitive Neuroscience of Organizations, 1118, 90–101.Google Scholar
  51. Tabibnia, G., Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2008). The sunny side of fairness - Preference for fairness activates reward circuitry (and disregarding unfairness activates self-control circuitry). Psychological Science, 19(4), 339–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Takahashi, H., Kato, M., Matsuura, M., Mobbs, D., Suhara, T., & Okubo, Y. (2009). When your gain is my pain and your pain is my gain: neural correlates of envy and schadenfreude. Science, 323(5916), 937–939.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and Validation of Brief Measures of Positive and Negative Affect - the Panas Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Watson, K. K., & Platt, M. L. (2008). Neuroethology of reward and decision making. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences, 363(1511), 3825–3835.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Williams, G. C., & Nesse, R. M. (1991). The dawn of Darwinian medicine. Quarterly Review of Biology, 66(1), 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Wirtz, D., Chiu, C. Y., Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2009). What constitutes a good life? Cultural differences in the role of positive and negative affect in subjective well-being. Journal of Personality, 77(4), 1167–1196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Wittchen, H. U., Jacobi, F., Rehm, J., Gustavsson, A., Svensson, M., Jonsson, B., et al. (2011). The size and burden of mental disorders and other disorders of the brain in Europe 2010. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 21(9), 655–679.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.OsloNorway

Personalised recommendations