Abstract
This paper examines the effect of culture on subjective well-being. By exploiting the natural experiment of migration we are able to separate the effect of culture (intrinsic cultural disposition, values, beliefs, norms) from other extrinsic institutional, economic and social factors. Using data from five rounds of the European Social Survey we find that holding constant the external environment (living in the same residence country) and controlling for the important socio-demographic attributes, immigrants from countries with high levels of life satisfaction report higher life satisfaction than immigrants from countries with low levels of life satisfaction. The effect of satisfaction in the birth country lasts across generations and is stronger for immigrants who are more attached to the culture of their birth country. Since any observed differences among the immigrants is their cultural background (their birth countries), the results can be interpreted as the effect of culture on life satisfaction. Our results suggest that besides economic and social variables, institutions and personal characteristics, cultural factors play an important role in satisfaction.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.


Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
We have to emphasize that although we analyze the determinants of immigrants’ life satisfaction, we use this special sample purely for a methodological reason: in this way we are able to isolate the effect of culture from external factors (institutions. policies, markets, etc.).
The country of origin was identified with the following question: "From what countries or part of the world did your ancestors come?”.
Arguably, genuine cultural impact is captured, caused by differences in values, attitudes and beliefs, and the results are free from cultural bias (different response styles or social desirability), or at least are not primarily driven by it (Hui and Triandis 1989; OECD 2013; van de Vijver and Poortinga 1997). Although it is hard or impossible to distinguish between cultural impact and cultural bias, the risk of cultural bias can be managed by careful survey design and translation process (OECD 2013). It has to be emphasized that the ESS tries to ensure optimal comparability and the equivalence of the different language versions of the questionnaires.
We include variables that are standard control variables in the empirical literature (Dolan et al. 2008; Frey and Stutzer 2002): age, squared age, female respondent, education [measured by International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED)], living with a partner, main activity, living in a city, activity limitation (hampered in daily activities by disability or any health problem), household's total net income in logarithmic form, relative income (ratio of household income to average income in the country), and household size.
Although our main interest is in life satisfaction, we checked the results using an alternative measure of well-being as well. Happiness was measured on an 11-point scale: “Taking all things together, how happy would you say you are?” Using this variable does not change the inference.
As mentioned above, average life satisfaction in the birth country depends on environmental and cultural factors, but we should not expect environment of the birth country to have a sizable effect on immigrants’ satisfaction in the residence country. Therefore, the positive coefficient on average life satisfaction in the birth country can be interpreted as the effect of the cultural component.
Moreover, Fernández (2011: 496) argues that “the epidemiological approach is biased towards finding that culture does not matter” since the effect of birth country culture is likely to diminish over time.
Excluding mean satisfaction in the birth country from the model, results for immigrants and natives are very similar, thus we suppose that the effect of culture would be the same for natives—if we were able to capture it.
Although omitted individual variables could be at least partially responsible for the observed effect of average satisfaction in the birth country if they differ systematically between immigrants from countries with high and low satisfaction levels, we show in the next section that the observed effect remains the same if we control for a richer set of individual characteristics. On the other hand, in line with the literature (Fernández and Fogli 2009; Luttmer and Singhal 2011), we assume that the economic, institutional and social environment of the birth country does not have any sizeable impact on the immigrants’ life satisfaction in their new country. In the next section, we also show that in a model controlling for important characteristics of the birth country the effect of satisfaction in the birth country does not change.
We use the integrated World Values Survey and European Values Survey data. We calculate the average life satisfaction in an immigrant’s birth country by computing the weighted mean satisfaction of all respondents in all six rounds of WVS and EVS (five rounds of WVS: 1981–1984, 1989–1993, 1994–1999, 1999–2004, 2005–2007, four rounds of EVS: 1981–1984, 1990–1993, 1999–2001, 2008–2010), and then taking the average of the six mean satisfactions.
The data on the share of foreign-born population come from OECD Factbook 2011. We use data for 2005 because for this year they were available for most of the countries.
The three questions (0–10 scales) are the following: “Would you say it is generally bad or good for [country]’s economy that people come to live here from other countries?”, “Would you say that [country]’s cultural life is generally undermined or enriched by people coming to live here from other countries?”, “Is [country] made a worse or a better place to live by people coming to live here from other countries?”
2851 individuals’ both parents emigrated from ESS countries, and 7321 individuals have a native parent and an immigrant parent. We calculate life satisfaction of the country of origin as the mean of the average satisfactions in the two parents’ birth countries, because with the mean of the satisfactions of the two parental birth countries we capture correctly the potential influence of the immigrant parent’s culture, but the direct effect of the native parent’s birth country culture is captured by residence country dummies.
In these specifications, we use a sample of native respondents with a mother from another ESS country (row 2), and a sample of native respondents with a father from another ESS country (row 3). Satisfaction in the parental birth country is calculated as average satisfaction in the mother’s (row 2) or in the father’s birth country (row 3). However, models with satisfaction in the parental birth country calculated like in row 1 of Table 5 (as the mean of the average satisfactions in the two parent’s birth countries) yields similar coefficient magnitudes.
References
Alesina, A., Di Tella, R., & MacCulloch, R. (2004). Inequality and happiness: Are Europeans and Americans different? Journal of Public Economics, 88(9–10), 2009–2042.
Alesina, A., & Giuliano, P. (2010). The power of the family. Journal of Economic Growth, 15(2), 93–125.
Algan, Y., & Cahuc, P. (2010). Inherited trust and growth. American Economic Review, 100(5), 2060–2092.
Askitas, N., & Zimmermann, K. F. (2011). Health and well-being in the crisis. IZA Discussion Paper, No. 5601.
Blau, F. D., Kahn, L. M., Liu, A. Y.-H., & Papps, K. L. (2013). The transmission of women’s fertility, human capital, and work orientation across immigrant generations. Journal of Population Economics, 26(2), 405–435.
Blau, F. D., Kahn, L. M., & Papps, K. L. (2011). Gender, source country characteristics, and labor market assimilation among immigrants. Review of Economics and Statistics, 93(1), 43–58.
Carroll, C. D., Rhee, B.-K., & Rhee, C. (1994). Are there cultural effects on saving? Some cross-sectional evidence. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 109(3), 685–699.
Casey, T., & Dustmann, C. (2010). Immigrants’ identity, economic outcomes and the transmission of identity across generations. Economic Journal, 120(542), F31–F51.
Deaton, A. (2008). Income, health, and well-being around the world: Evidence from the gallup world poll. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 22(2), 53–72.
Deaton, A. (2012). The financial crisis and the well-being of Americans: 2011 OEP Hicks Lecture. Oxford Economic Papers, 64(1), 1–26.
Di Tella, R., MacCulloch, R. J., & Oswald, A. J. (2003). The macroeconomics of happiness. Review of Economics and Statistics, 85(4), 809–827.
Diener, E. (Ed.). (2009). Culture and well-being: The collected works of Ed Diener. New York: Springer.
Diener, E., Diener, M., & Diener, C. (1995). Factors predicting the subjective well-being of nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5), 851–864.
Diener, E., Inglehart, R., & Tay, L. (2013). Theory and validity of life satisfaction scales. Social Indicators Research, 112(3), 497–527.
Diener, E., & Suh, E. M. (Eds.). (2000). Culture and subjective well-being. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125(2), 276–302.
Dinesen, P. T. (2013). where you come from or where you live? Examining the cultural and institutional explanation of generalized trust using migration as a natural experiment. European Sociological Review, 29(1), 114–128.
Dolan, P., Peasgood, T., & White, M. (2008). Do we really know what makes us happy? A review of the economic literature on the factors associated with subjective well-being. Journal of Economic Psychology, 29(1), 94–122.
Fernández, R. (2011). Does culture matter? In J. Benhabib, A. Bisin, & M. O. Jackson (Eds.), Handbook of social economics (Vol. 1, pp. 481–510). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Fernández, R., & Fogli, A. (2006). Fertility: The role of culture and family experience. Journal of the European Economic Association, 4(2–3), 552–561.
Fernández, R., & Fogli, A. (2009). Culture: An empirical investigation of beliefs, work, and fertility. American Economic Journal: Macroeconomics, 1(1), 146–177.
Frey, B. S. (2008). Happiness. A revolution in economics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Frey, B. S., & Stutzer, A. (2002). Happiness and economics: How the economy and institutions affect well-being. Princeton and Oxford: Princeton University Press.
Grosfeld, I., & Senik, C. (2010). The emerging aversion to inequality. Economics of Transition, 18(1), 1–26.
Guiso, L., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2006). Does culture affect economic outcomes? Journal of Economic Perspectives, 20(2), 23–48.
Hajdu, T., & Hajdu, G. (2014). Reduction of income inequality and subjective well-being in Europe. Economics: The Open-Access, Open-Assessment E-Journal, 8(35), 1–29.
Helliwell, J. F., & Putnam, R. D. (2004). The social context of well-being. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 359, 1435–1446.
Helliwell, J. F., & Wang, S. (2011). Trust and wellbeing. International Journal of Wellbeing, 1(1), 42–78.
Hui, C. H., & Triandis, H. C. (1989). Effects of culture and response format on extreme response style. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 20(3), 296–309.
Inglehart, R., & Klingemann, H.-D. (2000). Genes, culture, democracy and happiness. In E. Diener & E. M. Suh (Eds.), Culture and subjective well-being (pp. 165–183). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kasser, T., & Ahuvia, A. (2002). Materialistic values and well-being in business students. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 137–146.
Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of American dream: Correlates of financial success as a central life aspiration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(2), 410–422.
Knoop, H. H., & Delle Fave, A. (Eds.). (2013). Well-Being and cultures. Perspectives from positive psychology. Dordrecht, NL: Springer.
Luttmer, E. F. P., & Singhal, M. (2011). Culture, context, and the taste for redistribution. American Economic Journal: Economic Policy, 3(1), 157–179.
Lykken, D., & Tellegen, A. (1996). Happiness is a stochastic phenomenon. Psychological Science, 7(3), 186–189.
OECD. (2013). OECD guidelines on measuring subjective well-being. Paris: OECD Publishing.
Oishi, S. (2010). Culture and well-being: Conceptual and methodological issues. In E. Diener, J. F. Helliwell, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), International differences in well-being (pp. 34–69). New York: Oxford University Press.
Otrachshenko, V., & Popova, O. (2014). Life (dis)satisfaction and the intention to migrate: Evidence from central and eastern Europe. Journal of Socio-Economics, 48, 40–49.
Rice, T. W., & Steele, B. J. (2004). Subjective well-being and culture across time and space. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35(6), 633–647.
Senik, C. (2014). The French unhappiness puzzle: The cultural dimension of happiness. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 106, 379–401.
Stevenson, B., & Wolfers, J. (2013). Subjective well-being and income: Is there any evidence of satiation? American Economic Review, 103(3), 598–604.
Suh, E. M., & Koo, J. (2008). Comparing subjective well-being across cultures and nations. The “What” and “Why” Questions. In M. Eid & R. J. Larsen (Eds.), The science of subjective well-being (pp. 414–427). New York: The Guilford Press.
Suh, E. M., & Oishi, S. (2002). Subjective Well-Being Across Cultures. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, Unit 10. http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol10/iss1/1. Accessed November 24, 2011.
van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Poortinga, Y. H. (1997). Towards an integrated analysis of bias in cross-cultural assessment. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 13(1), 29–37.
Veenhoven, R. (1993). Happiness in nations: Subjective appreciation of life in 56 nations, 1946–1992. Erasmus University of Rotterdam, Department of Social Sciences, RISBO, Center for Socio-Cultural Transformation.
Veenhoven, R. (1994). Is happiness a trait? Social Indicators Research, 32(2), 101–160.
Veenhoven, R. (1996). Developments in satisfaction-research. Social Indicators Research, 37(1), 1–46.
Wolfers, J. (2003). Is business cycle volatility costly? Evidence from surveys of subjective well-being. International Finance, 6(1), 1–26.
Acknowledgments
We thank Gábor Kézdi, Matild Sági and Endre Sik for their comments, as well as participants in the HAS Institute for Sociology Young Researchers’ Conference (Budapest, December 2011), and the 11th ISQOLS Conference (Venice, November 2012). We also thank the useful comments of two anonymous reviewers. Remaining errors are solely ours.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Hajdu, G., Hajdu, T. The Impact of Culture on Well-Being: Evidence from a Natural Experiment. J Happiness Stud 17, 1089–1110 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-015-9633-9
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-015-9633-9