Skip to main content
Log in

Inverting the Logic of Economic Migration: Happiness Among Migrants Moving from Wealthier to Poorer Countries in Europe

  • Research Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Happiness Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Migration from a poorer country to a wealthier one often results in a lower relative economic status for the migrant (even when it increases their incomes in an “absolute” sense)—and thus perhaps results also in a decrease in his/her happiness. By the same logic, migration from a wealthy country to a poorer one might bring a higher status position for the migrant and so might raise his/her happiness. This paper investigates happiness among migrants who move from northern European countries to Spain, Portugal, Greece and Cyprus, comparing them to stayers in the origin countries (Belgium, Switzerland, France, Germany, Britain, and the Netherlands). The analysis shows that migrants are less happy than stayers, in a bivariate comparison and a conventional regression model. A consideration of results from “treatment models” and matching analyses suggests that the difference represents a decrease in happiness for the migrants (and not a difference in happiness prior to migration), contrary to an expectation rooted in an anticipated increase in economic status. Migrants have lower relative incomes than stayers; when relative income is controlled, the happiness disadvantage of migrants is smaller. Controlling additionally for absolute income does not lead to further change in that difference.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Stark and Taylor drew on a well-established sociological concept of relative deprivation (Merton 1968; Runciman 1966)—an idea that resonates strongly with the emphasis of happiness studies on relative income.

  2. The economic differences between the various European origin and destination countries are small compared to the larger international differences apparent at a global level. It is possible that the change in relative status for intra-European migrants is not sufficiently large to lead to a gain in happiness.

  3. Immigrants in the northern countries were removed from the dataset.

  4. In effect, then, the survey design implied that earning €300 a month in Romania was equivalent to earning €300 a month in France.

  5. As with most survey data, non-response rates for the income question are decidedly non-trivial, with roughly twenty per cent declining to answer in the sample considered here. The structure of non-response was explored using techniques of multiple imputation (Rubin 1987; Royston 2004). The data created via imputation provided no basis for believing that non-response was selective according to income level; for migrants there was no difference whatsoever in the mean, and for stayers the difference was trivial. The analysis below, then, reports results from the original data, without imputation (thus proceeding via listwise deletion).

  6. See Cong and Drucker (2001) for details of Stata’s “treatreg” routine and Maddala (1983) for the underlying model.

  7. Rates for vocational qualifications do not differ for the two comparisons, and so those who selected the vocational response are removed from these figures for presentational reasons.

  8. The specification here uses the more conservative “two-step” option in place of full-information maximum likelihood. Use of sample weights is then not possible.

  9. In regression analysis one might aspire to maximize r-squared, the proportion of variation explained by the model. That aspiration, in combination with the desire to avoid “omitted variable bias”, can lead one to add more control variables—perhaps without thinking clearly about what doing so means in the context of one’s research question.

  10. In a probit model of migration, all of those variables except gender are statistically significant at conventional levels.

  11. It might be disconcerting not to see a table providing more detailed information about these results—but the Stata output for matching analyses does not give any additional useful information (the only other figures reported are the standard error of the coefficient, the associated “z” and “p”, and a confidence interval).

References

  • Abadie, A., Drukker, D., Herr, J. L., & Imbens, G. W. (2004). Implementing matching estimators for average treatment effects in Stata. Stata Journal, 4(3), 290–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ball, R., & Chernova, K. (2008). Absolute income, relative income, and happiness. Social Indicators Research, 88(3), 497–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bălţătescu, S. (2007). Central and Eastern Europeans Migrants’ Subjective Quality of Life: A comparative study. Journal of Identity and Migration Studies, 1, 67–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartram, D. (2011). Economic migration and happiness: Comparing immigrants’ and natives’ happiness gains from income. Social Indicators Research, 103(1), 57–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartram, D. (2013). Happiness and “economic migration”: A comparison of Eastern European migrants and stayers. Migration Studies, 1(2), 156–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benson, M., & O’Reilly, K. (2009). Migration and the search for a better way of life: A critical exploration of lifestyle migration. The Sociological Review, 57(4), 608–625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cangiano, A., & Strozza, S. (2008). Foreign immigration in Southern European receiving countries: New evidence from national data sources. In C. Bonifa, M. Okolski, J. Schoorl, & P. Simon (Eds.), International migration in Europe (pp. 153–178). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chindarkar, N. (2014). Is subjective well-being of concern to potential migrants from Latin America? Social Indicators Research, 115(1), 159–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christoph, B. (2010). The relation between life satisfaction and the material situation: A re-evaluation using alternative measures. Social Indicators Research, 98(3), 475–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cong, R., & Drucker, D. M. (2001). Treatment effects model. Stata Technical Bulletin, 10, 25–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Lucas, R., Schimmack, U., & Helliwell, J. (2009). Well-being for public policy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dolan, P., Peasgood, T., & White, M. (2008). Do we really know what makes us happy? A review of the economic literature on the factors associated with subjective well-being. Journal of Economic Psychology, 29(1), 94–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, E. W., Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2011). If money doesn’t make you happy, then you probably aren’t spending it right. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21, 115–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, R. A. (1973). Does money buy happiness? Public Interest, 30(3), 3–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, R. A. (2001). Income and happiness: Towards a unified theory. The Economic Journal, 111(473), 465–484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erlinghagen, M. (2011). Nowhere better than here? The subjective well-being of German emigrants and remigrants. Comparative Population Studies, 36(4), 899–926.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferrer-i-Carbonell, A., & Frijters, P. (2004). How important is methodology for the estimates of the determinants of happiness? The Economic Journal, 114(497), 641–659.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Firebaugh, G., & Schroeder, M. B. (2009). Does your neighbor’s income affect your happiness? American Journal of Sociology, 115(3), 805–831.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, C. (2008). What wealth–happiness paradox? A short note on the American case. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9(2), 219–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelatt, J. (2013). Looking down or looking up: Status and subjective well-being among Asian and Latino immigrants in the United States. International Migration Review, 47(1), 39–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. (2006). Stumbling on happiness. New York: HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graham, C., & Markowitz, J. (2011). Aspirations and happiness of potential Latin American immigrants. Journal of Social Research and Policy, 2(2), 9–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guo, S., & Fraser, M. W. (2010). Propensity score analysis: Statistical methods and applications. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haybron, D. M. (2008). The pursuit of unhappiness: The elusive psychology of well-being. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jowell, R. (2007). European Social Survey, technical report. London: Centre for Comparative Social Surveys, City University.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, R., Warnes, T., & Williams, A. M. (2000). Sunset lives: British retirement migration to the Mediterranean. Oxford: Berg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knight, J., & Gunatilaka, R. (2010). Great expectations? The subjective well-being of rural–urban migrants in China. World Development, 38, 113–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Layard, R. (2005). Happiness: Lessons from a new science. New York: Penguin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maddala, G. S. (1983). Limited dependent and qualitative variables in economics. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Melzer, S. M. (2011). Does migration make you happy? The influence of migration on subjective well-being. Journal of Social Research and Policy, 2(2), 73–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merton, R. K. (1968). Social theory and social structure. New York: The Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, S. L., & Winship, C. (2007). Counterfactuals and causal inference: Methods and principles for social research. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Olgiati, A., Calvo, R., & Berkman, L. (2013). Are migrants going up a blind alley? Economic migration and life satisfaction around the world: Cross-national evidence from Europe, North America and Australia. Social Indicators Research, 114(2), 383–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piore, M. J. (1979). Birds of passage: Migrant labor and industrial societies (pp. x, 229). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Portes, A., & Bach, R. L. (1985). Latin journey: Cuban and Mexican immigrants in the United States. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratha, D., & Shaw, W. (2007). Southsouth migration and remittances. World Bank Development Prospects Group, Working Paper No. 102.

  • Royston, P. (2004). Multiple imputation of missing values. Stata Journal, 4(3), 227–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, D. B. (1987). Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys. New York: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Runciman, W. G. (1966). Relative deprivation and social justice: A study of attitudes to social inequality in 20th century England. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, D. W., Stevenson, B., & Wolfers, J. (2012). The new stylized facts about income and subjective well-being. Emotion, 12(6), 1181–1187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Safi, M. (2010). Immigrants’ life satisfaction in europe: Between assimilation and discrimination. European Sociological Review, 26(2), 159–171.

  • Stark, O., & Taylor, J. E. (1989). Relative deprivation and international migration. Demography, 26(1), 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, B., & Wolfers, J. (2008). Economic growth and subjective well-being: Reassessing the Easterlin paradox. IZA Discussion Paper 3654, Institute for the Study of Labor.

  • Stillman, S., Gibson, J., McKenzie, D., & Rohorua, H. (2013). Miserable migrants? Natural experiment evidence on international migration and objective and subjective well-being. World Development (forthcoming).

  • Veenhoven, R. (2008). Sociological theories of subjective well-being. In M. Eid & R. Larsen (Eds.), The science of subjective well-being: A tribute to Ed Diener (pp. 44–61). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Voicu, B., & Vasile, M. (2014). Do “cultures of life satisfaction” travel? Current Sociology, 62(1), 81–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, K. (2010). “It’s a limited kind of happiness’: Barriers to achieving human well-being among Peruvian migrants in London and Madrid. Bulletin of Latin American Research, 29(3), 367–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, K. (2011). Constructing migrant wellbeing: An exploration of life satisfaction amongst Peruvian migrants in London. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies, 37(9), 1459–1475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I am grateful to Marcy Brink-Danan for comments that helped to clarify the core idea for this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David Bartram.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bartram, D. Inverting the Logic of Economic Migration: Happiness Among Migrants Moving from Wealthier to Poorer Countries in Europe. J Happiness Stud 16, 1211–1230 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-014-9554-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-014-9554-z

Keywords

Navigation