Advertisement

Journal of Happiness Studies

, Volume 14, Issue 3, pp 795–815 | Cite as

Does Being Well-Off Make Us Happier? Problems of Measurement

  • Jiri Zuzanek
Research Paper

Abstract

The article uses General Social Survey data (GSS) collected by Statistics Canada from 1986 to 2005 and experience sampling data (ESM) collected in 1985 and 2003 at the University of Waterloo to examine relationships between economic growth, household income, and subjective sense of well-being. The article puts to a test two propositions made by Easterlin (Nations and households in economic growth: Essays in Honor of Moses Abramovitz. Academic Press, New York, NY,1974), namely that personal and household incomes correlate positively with subjective well-being, but this does not apply to the relationship between subjective well-being and societal economic growth. Analyses of GSS data reported in this article support Easterlin’s findings. They show that higher household incomes correlate positively with respondents’ retrospective assessments of life satisfaction, but economic growth has not been accompanied by a corresponding rise of subjective well-being. Analyses of ESM data suggest that when relationships between household income and subjective well-being are measured by “experiential” measures (Csikszentmihalyi and Larson in J Nerv Ment Dis 175: 526–537, 1987), these relationships are not statistically significant and subjective valuations of well-being taper off at the top of the income pyramid.

Keywords

GDP Happiness Income Measurement Subjective well-being 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank anonymous reviewer # 1 for a series of constructive suggestions and Alexander Graham for assistance in preparing the manuscript of this article.

References

  1. Abramovitz, M. (1959). The welfare interpretation of secular trends in national income and product. In M. Abramovitz, et al. (Eds.), The allocation of economic resources: Essays in honor of Bernard Francis Haley. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  2. Alliger, G. M., & Williams, K. J. (1993). Using signal-contingent experience sampling methodology to study work in the field: A discussion and illustration examining task perceptions and mood. Personnel Psychology, 46(3), 525–549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Andrews, F. M., & Whitey, S. B. (1976). Social indicators of well-being in America. New York, NY: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Aristotle. (2009). The politics and the constitution of Athens. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Balzac, H. (1838) La Maison Nucingen, http://www.feedbooks.com/book/1901/la-maison-nucingen.
  6. Blanchflower, D. G., & Oswald, A. (2004). Well-being overtime in Britain and the USA. Journal of Public Economics, 88, 1359–1386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brooker, A. S. & Hyman, I. (2010). Time Use. A Report of the Canadian Index of Wellbeing (CIW). Toronto.Google Scholar
  8. Campbell, A. (1972). Aspiration, satisfaction and fulfillment. In A. Campbell & P. Converse (Eds.), The human meaning of social change (pp. 441–446). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
  9. Campbell, A., Converse, P., & Rodgers, W. (1976). The quality of American life. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
  10. Cantril, A. H., & Roll, C. W., Jr. (1971). Hopes and fears of the American people. New York: Universe.Google Scholar
  11. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. (Eds.). (1988). Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Hunter, J. (2003). Happiness in everyday life: The uses of experience sampling. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4, 185–199.Google Scholar
  13. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reliability of the experience sampling method. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175(9), 526–537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Deaton, A. (2008). Income, health, and well-being around the world: Evidence from the Gallup World Poll. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 22(2), 53–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. DeVoe, S. E., & House, J. (2011). Time, money, and happiness: How does putting a price on time affect our ability to smell the roses? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.11.012.
  16. Diener, E., Helliwell, J. F., & Kahneman, D. (Eds.). (2010). International differences in well-being. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Diener, E., Sandvik, E., Seidlitz, L., & Diener, M. (1993). The relationship between income and subjective well-being: Relative or absolute? Social Indicators Research, 28, 195–223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Diener, E., & Suh, E. M. (1999). National differences in subjective well-being. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 434–450). New York, NY: Russell Sage.Google Scholar
  19. Dow, G. K., & Juster, F. T. (1985). Goods, time and well-being: The joint dependence problem. In F. T. Juster & F. P. Stafford (Eds.), Time, goods, and well-being (pp. 397–413). Ann Arbor, MI: Survey Research Center, Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan.Google Scholar
  20. Drewnowski, J. (1970). Studies in the measurement of levels of living and welfare. Geneva: United Nations Research Institute for Social Development.Google Scholar
  21. Duesenberry, J. S. (1952). Income, saving and the theory of consumer behaviour. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  22. Durkheim, E. (1893). The division of labour in society. New York, NY: The Macmillan Company 1930.Google Scholar
  23. Easterlin, R. A. (1974). Does economic growth improve the human lot? Some empirical evidence. In P. A. David & M. W. Reder (Eds.), Nations and households in economic growth: Essays in Honor of Moses Abramovitz (pp. 98–125). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  24. Easterlin, R. A. (1995). Will raising the income of all increase the happiness of all? Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 27(1), 35–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Easterlin, R.A. (2004) Feeding the illusion of growth and happiness: A reply to Hagerty and Veenhoven. http://www-bcf.usc.edu/~easterl/papers/HVcomment.pdf.
  26. Easterlin, R.A. & Angelescu, L. (2009). Happiness and growth the world over: Time series evidence on the happiness-income paradox. IZA Discussion Paper (4060).Google Scholar
  27. Easterlin, R. A., Angelescu, L., Switek, M., Sawangfa, O. & Zweig, J. S. (2010). The happiness-income paradox revisited. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (PNAS), (doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015962107).
  28. Erikson, R. (1974). Welfare as a planning goal. Acta Sociologica, 17(3), 273–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Franklin, B. (1748) Advice to Young Tradesman Written by an Old One. Philadelphia: New Printing Office.Google Scholar
  30. Galbraith, J. K. (1958). The affluent society. Boston: Houghton.Google Scholar
  31. Gershuny, J. (2011). Time-use surveys and the measurement of national well-being. Oxford: University of Oxford, Centre for Time Use Research.Google Scholar
  32. Graham, C. (2009). Happiness around the world. The paradox of happy peasants and miserable millionaires. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Graham, C. (2011). Does more money make you happier? Why so much debate? Applied Research Quality Life, 6, 219–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hagerty, M. R. (2000). Social comparison of income in one’s community. Evidence from National Surveys of income and happiness. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 78(4), 764–771.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Hagerty, M. R., & Veenhoven, R. (2003). Wealth and happiness revisited—Growing national income does go with greater happiness. Social Indicators Research, 64, 1–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Hektner, J. M., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Schmidt, J. (2002). Experience Sampling Method: Measuring the Quality of Everyday Life. Google Scholar
  37. Helliwell, J. F. (2011). How can subjective well-being be improved? http://www.earth.columbia.edu/bhutan-conference-2011/sitefiles/John%2520Helliwell%2520-%2.
  38. Homans, G. C. (1961). Social behavior: Its elementary forms. New York, NY: Harcourt.Google Scholar
  39. Hurlburt, R. T., Lech, B. C., & Saltman, S. (1984). Random sampling of thought and mood. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8, 263–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Juster, F. T. (1985). The validity and quality of time use estimates obtained from recall diaries. In F. T. Juster & F. P. Stafford (Eds.), Time, goods, and well-being (pp. 63–91). Ann Arbor, MI: Survey Research Center, Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan.Google Scholar
  41. Juster, F. T. (1990). Rethinking utility theory. The Journal of Behavioral Economics, 19(2), 155–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Juster, F. T., Courant, P. N., & Dow, G. K. (1981). A theoretical framework for the measurement of well-being. The Review of Income and Wealth, Series, 27(1), 1–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Juster, F. T., Ono, H., & Stafford, F. P. (2003). An assessment of alternative measures of time use. Sociological Methodology, 33(1), 19–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Kahneman, D., & Deaton, A. (2010). Does money buy happiness….or just a better life. Princeton: Princeton University. Mimeo.Google Scholar
  45. Kahneman, D., & Krueger, A. B. (2006). Developments in the measurement of subjective well-being. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 20(1), 2–24.Google Scholar
  46. La Rochefoucauld, Francois. (1644). Réflexions ou sentences et maximes morales http://www.gutenberg.org/ebooks/14913.
  47. Layard, R. (2005). Happiness: Lessons from a new science. New York, NY: Penguin Press.Google Scholar
  48. Merton, R. K. (1949). Social theory and social structure. New York, NY: Free Press.Google Scholar
  49. Noll, H. (2002). Social indicators and quality of life research: Background, achievements and current trends. In N. Genov (Ed.), Advances in sociological knowledge over half a century. Paris: International Social Science Council.Google Scholar
  50. Robinson, J. (1977). How Americans use time. A social-psychological analysis of everyday behavior. New York, NY: Praeger.Google Scholar
  51. Robinson, J. P. (1987). Microbehavioral approaches to monitoring human experience. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorders, 175(9), 514–518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Sacks, D. W., Stevenson, B. & Wolfers, J. (2010). Subjective well-being, income, economic development and growth. NBER Working Paper Series, (16441).Google Scholar
  53. Shakespeare, W. (1623) As you like it. http://www.gutenberg.org/cache/epub/1121/pg1121.txt.
  54. Stevenson, B. & Wolfers, J. (2008) Economic growth and subjective well-being: Reassessing the Easterlin Paradox. http://bpp.wharton.upenn.edu/jwolfers/Papers/EasterlinParadox.pdf.
  55. Stone, A. A., & Shiffman, S. (1992). Reflecting on the intensive measurement of stress, coping, and mood, with an emphasis on daily measures. Psychology and Health, 7, 115–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Strumilin, S. G. (1922) Problemy ekonomiki truda (Problems of the Economics of Labour). Moskva: Nauka (first published in 1922).Google Scholar
  57. Stutzer, A. (2002) The Role of Income Aspirations in Individual Happiness. Working Papers Series ISSN 1424-0459, Institute of Empirical research in Economics, University of Zurich.Google Scholar
  58. Tocqueville, A. (1856). The old regime and the revolution. New York, NY: Harper and Brothers.Google Scholar
  59. Van den Haag, E. (1957). Of happiness and of despair we have no measure. In: Rosenberg B. & White D. M. Eds. Mass Culture. The Popular Arts in America. New York, NY: The Free Press of Glencoe (pp. 504–536).Google Scholar
  60. Van Praag, B., & Ferrer-i-Carbonell, A. (2004). Happiness quantified: Satisfaction calculus approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Veenhoven, R. (1991). Is happiness relative? Social Indicators Research, 24(1), 1–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Zuzanek, J. (2005). Adolescent time use and well-being from a comparative perspective. Loisir and Societe/Society and Leisure, 28(2), 379–423.Google Scholar
  63. Zuzanek, J. (2009) Time use imbalance: Developmental and emotional costs. In: K. Matuska & Ch. R. Christiansen (Eds.) Life balance. Multidisciplinary theories and research. Bethesda MD: AOTA Press.Google Scholar
  64. Zuzanek, J., & Mannell, R. (1993). Leisure behaviour and experiences as part of everyday life: The weekly rhythm. Loisir and Societe/Society and Leisure, 16(1), 31–57.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Faculty of Applied Health SciencesUniversity of WaterlooWaterlooCanada

Personalised recommendations