Abstract
Overconfidence, a widely observed cognitive bias, has been linked to increased gambling motivations and behaviors. However, previous studies have largely overlooked overconfidence under a social comparison context, known as overplacement, i.e., the tendency of individuals to believe that they are better than their similar peers. In the present study, we tested the effect of overplacement on gambling motivations and behaviors though a Pilot Survey of Chinese college students (N = 129) and a Field Survey of Chinese Macao casino gamblers (N = 733). Our results revealed a double-edged sword effect of overplacement: Serving as a risk factor, evaluating one self’s earning ability as higher than others was linked to more gambling motivations (β = 0.18, p = .005) and frequency (β = 0.18, p = .004); Serving as a protective factor, evaluating oneself as happier than others was linked to less gambling motivations (β = − 0.32, p < .001) and problem behaviors (β = − 0.26, p < .001). These findings expand the relationship between overconfidence and gambling from a cognitive bias perspective to a social comparison perspective. Our study not only revealed a typical profile of gambling motivations and behaviors among different demographic groups in Chinese casino gamblers, but also highlighted the importance of considering social factors in the study of the psychological mechanisms of gambling.
Similar content being viewed by others
Data Availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author.
Notes
These games included Fan-tan, baccarat, greyhound racing, cussec, football lottery, paikao, horse racing, blackjack, roulette, Chinese lottery, mahjong, stud poker, and slot machines. Please refer to the DICJ web site for detailed information, http://www.dicj.gov.mo/web/en/rules/index.html.
References
Alicke, M. D., & Sedikides, C. (2011). Handbook of self-enhancement and self-protection. Guilford.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR).
Bruhin, A., Santos-Pinto, L., & Staubli, D. (2018). How do beliefs about skill affect risky decisions? Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 150, 350–371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2018.01.016.
Calado, F., & Griffiths, M. D. (2016). Problem gambling worldwide: An update and systematic review of empirical research (2000–2015). Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 5(4), 592–613. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.5.2016.073.
Camchong, J., Goodie, A. S., Mcdowell, J. E., Gilmore, C. S., & Clementz, B. A. (2007). A cognitive neuroscience approach to studying the role of overconfidence in problem gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 23(2), 185–199. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-006-9033-5.
Chen, J. H., Tong, K. K., Wu, A. M., Lau, J. T., & Zhang, M. X. (2018). The comorbidity of gambling disorder among Macao adult residents and the moderating role of resilience and life purpose. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15, e2774. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15122774.
Ciccarelli, M., Malinconico, R., Griffiths, M. D., Nigro, G., & Cosenza, M. (2016). Reward preferences of pathological gamblers under conditions of uncertainty: An experimental study. Journal of Gambling Studies, 32(4), 1175–1189. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-016-9593-y.
Clark, L. (2010). Decision-making during gambling: An integration of cognitive and psychobiological approaches. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences, 365(1538), 319–330. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0147.
Cosenza, M., Griffiths, M. D., Nigro, G., & Ciccarelli, M. (2017). Risk-taking, delay discounting, and time perspective in adolescent gamblers: An experimental study. Journal of Gambling Studies, 33(2), 383–395. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-016-9623-9.
Dufner, M., Gebauer, J. E., Sedikides, C., & Denissen, J. J. A. (2018). Self-enhancement and psychological adjustment: A meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 23(1), 48–72. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868318756467.
Farrell, L. (2018). Understanding the relationship between subjective well-being and gambling behavior. Journal of Gambling Studies, 34, 55–71. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-017-9692-4.
Field, A. P., & Wilcox, R. R. (2017). Robust statistical methods: A primer for clinical psychology and experimental psychopathology researchers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 98, 19–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2017.05.013.
Goodie, A. S. (2005). The role of perceived control and overconfidence in pathological gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 21(4), 481–502. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-005-5559-1.
Gordon, R., & Reith, G. (2019). Gambling as social practice: A complementary approach for reducing harm? Harm Reduction Journal, 16(1), e64. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12954-019-0342-2.
Hubert, P., & Griffiths, M. D. (2018). A comparison of Online Versus Offline Gambling Harm in Portuguese pathological gamblers: An empirical study. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 16(5), 1219–1237. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-017-9846-8.
Jacobsen, L. H., Knudsen, A. K., Krogh, E., Pallesen, S., & Molde, H. (2007). An overview of cognitive mechanisms in pathological gambling. Nordic Psychology, 59(4), 347–361.
Johansson, A., Grant, J. E., Kim, S. W., Odlaug, B. L., & Götestam, K. G. (2009). Risk factors for problematic gambling: A critical literature review. Journal of Gambling Studies, 25(1), 67–92.
Kaplan, B. A., Amlung, M., Reed, D. D., Jarmolowicz, D. P., & Lemley, S. M. (2016). Automating scoring of delay discounting for the 21- and 27-item monetary choice questionnaires. Behavior Analyst, 39(2), 293–304. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-016-0070-9.
Kirby, K. N., & MarakoviĆ, N. N. (1996). Delay-discounting probabilistic rewards: Rates decrease as amounts increase. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(1), 100–104. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03210748.
Klar, Y., & Giladi, E. (1999). Are most people happier than their peers, or are they just happy? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 586–595. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167299025005004.
Koller, M., & Stahel, W. A. (2011). Sharpening Wald-type inference in robust regression for small samples. Computational Statistics & Data Analysis, 55(8), 2504–2515. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.csda.2011.02.014.
Kwak, & Hee, D. (2016). The overestimation phenomenon in a skill-based gaming bontext: The case of March madness pools. Journal of Gambling Studies, 32(1), 107–123. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-015-9520-7.
Lai, D. (2006). Gambling and the older Chinese in Canada. Journal of Gambling Studies, 22, 121–141. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-005-9006-0.
Langer, E. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 311–328. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.32.2.311.
Larrick, R. P., Burson, K. A., & Soll, J. B. (2007). Social comparison and confidence: When thinking you’re better than average predicts overconfidence (and when it does not). Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102(1), 76–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2006.10.002.
Ledgerwood, D. M., Alessi, S. M., Phoenix, N., & Petry, N. M. (2009). Behavioral assessment of impulsivity in pathological gamblers with and without substance use disorder histories versus healthy controls. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 105(1–2), 89–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.06.011.
Li, S., Zhou, K., Sun, Y., Rao, L. L., Zheng, R., & Liang, Z. Y. (2010). Anticipated regret, risk perception, or both: Which is most likely responsible for our intention to gamble? Journal of Gambling Studies, 26(1), 105–116. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-009-9149-5.
Linde, J., & Sonnemans, J. (2012). Social comparison and risky choices. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 44(1), 45–72. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11166-011-9135-z.
Liu, Z., Elliot, A. J., & Li, Y. (2021). Social comparison orientation and trait competitiveness: Their interrelation and utility in predicting overall and domain-specific risk-taking. Personality and Individual Differences, 171, 110451. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110451.
Loo, J. M. Y., Raylu, N., & Oei, T. P. S. (2008). Gambling among the Chinese: A comprehensive review. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(7), 1152–1166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2008.04.001.
Măirean, C., & Havârneanu, C. E. (2018). The relationship between drivers’ illusion of superiority, aggressive driving, and self-reported risky driving behaviors. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 55, 167–174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2018.02.037.
Meertens, R. M., & Lion, R. (2010). Measuring an individual’s tendency to take risks: The risk propensity scale. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 38(6), 1506–1520. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2008.00357.x.
Mishra, S., Lalumière, M. L., & Williams, R. J. (2010). Gambling as a form of risk-taking: Individual differences in personality, risk-accepting attitudes, and behavioral preferences for risk. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(6), 616–621. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.032.
Moore, D., & Healy, P. (2008). The trouble with overconfidence. Psychological Review, 115, 502–517. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.502.
Muggleton, N., Parpart, P., Newall, P., Leake, D., Gathergood, J., & Stewart, N. (2021). The association between gambling and financial, social and health outcomes in big financial data. Nature Human Behaviour, 5(3), 319–326. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-01045-w.
Myerson, J., Green, L., & Warusawitharana, M. (2001). Area under the curve as a measure of discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 76(2), 235–243. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2001.76-235.
Nigro, G., & Cosenza, M. (2016). Living in the now: Decision-making and delay discounting in adolescent gamblers. Journal of Gambling Studies, 32(4), 1191–1202. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-016-9595-9.
Oei, T. P., & Goh, Z. (2014). Interactions between risk and protective factors on problem gambling in Asia. Journal of Gambling Studies, 31, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-013-9440-3.
Orford, J., Wardle, H., & Griffiths, M. (2013). What proportion of gambling is problem gambling? Estimates from the 2010 British Gambling Prevalence Survey. International Gambling Studies, 13(1), 4–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2012.689001.
Pallier, G., Wilkinson, R., Danthiir, V., Kleitman, S., & Roberts, R. D. (2002). The role of individual differences in the accuracy of confidence judgments. Journal of General Psychology, 129(3), 257–299. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221300209602099.
Petry, N. M., Ginley, M. K., & Rash, C. J. (2017). A systematic review of treatments for problem gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 31(8), 951–961. https://doi.org/10.1037/adb0000290.
Raylu, N., & Oei, T. P. (2004). The Gambling related cognitions Scale (GRCS): Development, confirmatory factor validation and psychometric properties. Addiction, 99, 757–769. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00753.x.
Salonen, A. H., Hellman, M., Latvala, T., & Castrén, S. (2018). Gambling participation, gambling habits, gambling-related harm, and opinions on gambling advertising in Finland in 2016. Nordic Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 35(3), 215–234. https://doi.org/10.1177/1455072518765875.
Schüll, N. D. (2012). Addiction by design. Princeton University Press.
Spohn, D. L., Devore-Suazo, I. F., Bernarding, M. L., & Güss, C. D. (2022). The role of social context in risky decision-making: Presence of friend and low resistance to peer influence increase risky decision-making. International Journal of Psychology, n/ahttps://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12864.
Stewart, S. H., & Zack, M. (2010). Development and psychometric evaluation of a three-dimensional gambling motives Questionnaire. Addiction, 103(7), 1110–1117. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02235.x.
Tang, U., & Sheng, N. (2009). Macao.Cities, 26(4), 220–231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cities.2009.02.005
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103(2), 193–210. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.103.2.193.
Thompson, S., Armstrong, W., & Thomas, C. (1998). Illusions of control, underestimations, and accuracy: A control heuristic explanation. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 143–161. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.123.2.143.
Toneatto, T., Blitz-Miller, T., Calderwood, K., Dragonetti, R., & Tsanos, A. (1997). Cognitive distortions in heavy gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 13(3), 253–266. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024983300428.
Tsukayama, E., & Duckworth, A. L. (2010). Domain-specific temporal discounting and temptation. Judgment and Decision Making, 5(2), 72–82. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1930297500000930.
Un, J., & Lam, D. (2016). The portrayal of gambling and cognitive biases in Chinese gambling-themed movies. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 14(2), 200–216. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-015-9563-0.
Wang, Y., Zheng, L., Wang, C., & Guo, X. (2020). Attenuated self-serving bias in people with internet gaming disorder is related to altered neural activity in subcortical-cortical midline structures. Bmc Psychiatry, 20, e512. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-020-02914-4.
Weber, E. U., Blais, A. R., & Betz, N. E. (2002). A domain-specific risk-attitude scale: Measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15(4), 263–290. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.414.
Welte, J. W., Barnes, G. M., Tidwell, M. C. O., & Hoffman, J. H. (2011). Gambling and problem gambling across the lifespan. Journal of Gambling Studies, 27(1), 49–61. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-010-9195-z.
Welte, J. W., Barnes, G. M., Tidwell, M. C. O., & Wieczorek, W. F. (2017). Predictors of problem gambling in the U.S. Journal of Gambling Studies, 33(2), 327–342. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-016-9639-1.
Williams, R. J., Volberg, R. A., & Stevens, R. M. (2012). The population prevalence of problem gambling: Methodological influences, standardized rates, jurisdictional differences, and worldwide trends. Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/10133/3068.
Wills, T. (1981). Downward comparison principles in social psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 245–271. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.90.2.245.
Wojcik, S., & Ditto, P. (2014). Motivated happiness: Self-enhancement inflates self-reported subjective well-being. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5, 825–834. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550614534699.
Wong, I., & So, E. (2003). Prevalence estimates of problem and pathological gambling in Hong Kong. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1353–1354. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.160.7.1353.
Zell, E., Strickhouser, J. E., Sedikides, C., & Alicke, M. D. (2020). The better-than-average effect in comparative self-evaluation: A comprehensive review and meta-analysis [American Psychological Association https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000218]. Retrieved.
Zeng, Z., Wang, X., & Wong, I. (2020). A study of adaptive gambling behaviors of visitors from Mainland China to Macau. Asian Education and Development Studies, ahead-of-print, https://doi.org/10.1108/AEDS-02-2020-0028.
Zhou, K., Tang, H., Sun, Y., Huang, G. H., Rao, L. L., Liang, Z. Y., & Li, S. (2012). Belief in luck or in skill: Which locks people into gambling? Journal of Gambling Studies, 28(3), 379–391. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-011-9263-z.
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (71761167001, 72271230), Major Projects of the National Social Science Foundation of China (No. 19ZDA358).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Yuepei Xu performed statistical analysis and drafted the manuscript; Gui-Hai Huang designed and collected data for the field survey; Yi Xiao collected data for the pilot survey and drafted part of the manuscript; Wei Wang conducted statistical analysis; Shu Li designed the field survey; and Zhu-Yuan Liang designed and coordinated the study, revised the manuscript, and provided supervision.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing Interests
The authors declare that there are no conflict and/or competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic Supplementary Material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
About this article
Cite this article
Xu, Y., Huang, GH., Xiao, Y. et al. A Double-Edged-Sword Effect of Overplacement: Social Comparison Bias Predicts Gambling Motivations and Behaviors in Chinese Casino Gamblers. J Gambl Stud 40, 1–20 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-024-10293-8
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-024-10293-8