Skip to main content
Log in

Understanding Within-Session Loss-Chasing: An Experimental Investigation of the Impact of Stake Size on Cognitive Control

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Gambling Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Loss-chasing is a central feature of problematic gambling, yet it remains a poorly conceived and understood concept. Loss-chasing is believed to stem from an erosion of cognitive control when gambling. The opportunity to gamble at significantly disparate stake sizes on a gambling activity is considered to be a risk factor for loss-chasing. This study investigated the impact of gambling at disparate stake sizes on executive processes integral to maintaining cognitive control when gambling, namely response inhibition and reflection impulsivity. Frequent adult non-problem gamblers (n = 32) participated in a repeated measures experiment; and gambled at three disparate stake sizes (£20, £2 and no stake per bet) on a simulated gambling task. Participants’ response inhibition performance and reflection impulsivity levels after gambling at various stake sizes were compared via a go/no-go task and information sampling task, respectively. Quality of decision-making i.e. the evaluation of available information to make probability judgements was impaired after gambling at higher stakes in comparison to lower stakes, indicating an increase in reflection impulsivity. No effect on response inhibition was observed. Although exploratory, this suggests that the opportunity for participants to substantially increase stake size on a gambling activity may be a risk factor for impaired cognitive performance when gambling, and perhaps create vulnerability for within-session loss-chasing in some players.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.).doi:10.1176/appi.books.9780890423349

  • Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioural inhibition, sustained attention and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65–94. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Belin, D., Mar, A. C., Dalley, J. W., Robbins, T. W., & Everitt, B. J. (2008). High impulsivity predicts the switch to compulsive cocaine-taking. Science, 320, 1352–1355. doi:10.1126/science.1158136.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Billieux, J., Lagrange, G., Van der Linden, M., Lançon, C., Adida, M., & Jeanningros, R. (2012). Investigation of impulsivity in a sample of treatment-seeking pathological gamblers: A multidimensional perspective. Psychiatry Research, 198(2), 291–296. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2012.01.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blaszczynski, A. (2013). A critical examination of the link between gaming machines and gambling related harm. Journal of Gambling Business and Economics, 7(3), 55–76. doi:10.5750/jgbe.v7i3.818.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blaszczynski, A., & Nower, L. (2002). A pathways model of problem and pathological gambling. Addiction, 97(5), 487–499.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Block, J., Block, J. H., & Harrington, D. M. (1974). Some misgivings about the matching familiar figures test as a measure of reflection impulsivity. Developmental Psychology, 10, 611–632.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braverman, J., & Shaffer, H. (2010). How do gamblers start gambling: Identifying behavioural markers for high-risk internet gambling. European Journal of Public Health, 22(2), 273–278. doi:10.1093/eurpub/ckp232.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Breen, R. B., & Zuckerman, M. (1999). Chasing in gambling behavior: Personality and cognitive determinants. Personality and Individual Differences, 92, 1097–1111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell-Meiklejohn, D. K., Woolrich, M. W., Passingham, R. E., & Rogers, R. D. (2008). Knowing when to stop: The brain mechanisms of chasing losses. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 293–300. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, L., Crooks, B., Clarke, R., Aitken, M. R. F., & Dunn, B. D. (2012). Physiological responses to near-miss outcomes and personal control during simulated gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 28(1), 123–137. doi:10.1007/s10899-011-9247-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, L., Lawrence, A., Astley-Jones, F., & Gray, N. (2009a). Gambling near-misses enhance motivation to gamble and recruit win-related brain circuitry. Neuron, 61(3), 481–490. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.031.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, L., Robbins, T. W., Ersche, K. D., & Sahakian, B. J. (2006). Reflection impulsivity in current and former substance users. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 515–522. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.11.007.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, L., Roiser, J. P., Robbins, T. W., & Sahakian, B. J. (2009b). Disrupted reflection impulsivity in cannabis users but not current or former ecstasy users. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 23, 14–22. doi:10.1177/0269881108089587.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Coates, E., & Blaszczynski, A. (2013). Predictors of return rate discrimination in slot machine play. Journal of Gambling Studies, 29(4), 631–645. doi:10.1007/s10899-013-9375-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, L. J., Nesci, C., Steinfel, M., Haeri, S., & Galynker, I. (2010). Investigating the relationship between sexual and chemical addictions by comparing executive function in subjects with pedophilia or opiate addiction and healthy controls. Journal of Psychiatry Practice, 16, 405–412. doi:10.1097/01.pra.0000390759.04581.7c.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conversano, C., Marazziti, D., Carmassi, C., Baldini, S., Barnabei, G., & Dell’Osso, L. (2012). Pathological gambling: a systematic review of biochemical, neuroimaging and neuropsychological findings. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 20, 130–148. doi:10.3109/10673229.2012.694318.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corless, T., & Dickerson, M. (1989). Gamblers’ self-perceptions of the determinants of impaired control. British Journal of Addiction, 84, 1527–1537.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Delfabbro, P. H., Falzon, K., & Ingram, T. (2005). The effects of parameter variations in electronic gambling simulations: Results of a laboratory-based pilot study. Gambling Research, 17, 7–25. doi:10.1002/9781118316078.ch4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 225–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dickerson, M., Hinchy, J., & Fabre, J. (1987). Chasing, arousal and sensation seeking in off-course gamblers. British Journal of Addiction, 82, 673–680.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, M. R., Maclin, O. H., & Daugherty, D. (2006). An evaluation of response allocations to concurrently available slot machine simulations. Behavior Research Methods, 38(2), 232–236. doi:10.1037/a0023630.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferris, J., & Wynne, H. (2001). The Canadian problem gambling index: Final report. Submitted for the Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse.

  • Fillmore, M. T. (2003). Drug abuse as a problem of impaired control: Current approaches and findings. Behavioral & Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 2, 179–197. doi:10.1177/1534582303257007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, R. Z., & Volkow, N. D. (2002). Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: Neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(10), 1642–1652. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.159.10.1642.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Goudriaan, A. E., Oosterlaan, J., deBeurs, E., & van den Brink, W. (2006). Neurocognitive functions in pathological gambling: A comparison with alcohol dependence, Tourette syndrome and normal controls. Addiction, 101, 534–547. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01380.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goudriaan, A. E., Yucel, M., & van Holst, R. J. (2014). Getting a grip on problem gambling: What can neuroscience tell us? Frontiers in Behavioural Neuroscience, 8, 141. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hester, R., & Garavan, H. (2005). Working memory and executive function: the influence of content and load on the control of attention. Memory & Cognition, 33, 221–233. doi:10.3758/BF03195311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, E. A., Craske, M. G., & Graybiel, A. M. (2014). Psychological treatments: A call for mental-health science. Nature, 511, 287–289. doi:10.1038/511287a.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kertzman, S., Lowengrub, K., Aizer, A., Vainder, M., Kotler, M., & Dannon, P. (2008). Go-no-go performance in pathological gamblers. Psychiatry Research, 161, 1–10. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2007.06.026.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kräplin, A., Dshemuchadse, M., Behrendt, S., Scherbaum, S., Goschke, T., & Bühringer, G. (2014). Dysfunctional decision-making in pathological gambling: Pattern specificity and the role of impulsivity. Psychiatry Research, 215(3), 675–682. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.041.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lawrence, A. J., Luty, J., Bogdan, N. A., Sahakian, B. J., & Clark, L. (2009a). Impulsivity and response inhibition in alcohol dependence and problem gambling. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 207(1), 163–172. doi:10.1007/s00213-009-1645-x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lawrence, A. J., Luty, J., Bogdan, N. A., Sahakian, B. J., & Clark, L. (2009b). Problem gamblers share deficits in impulsive decision-making with alcohol-dependent individuals. Addiction, 104(6), 1006–1015. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02533.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lesiuer, H. R. (1979). The compulsive gambler’s spiral of options and involvement. Psychiatry, 42, 79–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Livingstone, C., Wooley, R., Zazryn, T., Bakacs, L., & Shami, R. (2008). The relevance and role of gaming machine games and game features on the play of problem gamblers. In Report for the Independent Gambling Authority (IGA). Adelaide: South Australia Independent Gambling Authority. http://www.iga.sa.gov.au/pdf/0801/Final%20report.Print.Feb08.pdf

  • O’Connor, J., & Dickerson, M. (2003). Definition and measurement of chasing in off-course betting and gaming machine play. Journal of Gambling Studies, 19, 359–386. doi:10.1046/j.1360-0443.2003.00232.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parke, J., & Parke, A. (2013). Does size really matter? A review of the role of stake and prize levels in relation to gambling related harm. Journal of Gambling Business and Economics, 7(3), 77–110. doi:10.5750/jgbe.v7i3.819.

    Google Scholar 

  • Potenza, M. N. (2014). The neural bases of cognitive processes in gambling disorder. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(8), 429–438. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.03.007.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Quednow, B. B., Kuhn, K. U., Hoppe, C., Westheide, J., Maier, W., Daum, I., & Wagner, M. (2007). Elevated impulsivity and impaired decision-making cognition in heavy users of MDMA. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 189, 517–530. doi:10.1007/s00213-005-0256-4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sharpe, L. (2002). A reformulated cognitive behavioral model of problem gambling. A biopsychosocial perspective. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 1–25. doi:10.1016/S0272-7358(00)00087-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sharpe, L., Walker, M., Coughlan, M. J., Enersen, K., & Blaszczynski, A. (2005). Structural changes to electronic gaming machines as effective harm minimization strategies for non-problem and problem gamblers. Journal of Gambling Studies, 21(4), 503–520. doi:10.1007/s10899-005-5560-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spada, M. A., & Roarty, A. (2015). The relative contribution of metacognitions and attentional control to the severity of gambling in problem gamblers. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 1, 7–11. doi:10.1016/j.abrep.2015.02.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, T., Brevers, D., Chambers, C. D., Lavric, A., McLaren, I. P. L., Mertens, M., et al. (2015). How does response inhibition influence decision making when gambling? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 21(1), 15–36. doi:10.1037/xap0000039.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Strakowski, S. M., Fleck, D. E., DelBello, M. P., Adler, C. M., Shear, P. K., McElroy, S. L., et al. (2009). Characterising impulsivity in mania. Bipolar Disorders, 11, 41–51. doi:10.1111/j.1399-5618.2008.00658.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • van Holst, R. J., van den Brink, W., Veltman, D. J., & Goudriaan, A. E. (2010). Why gamblers fail to win: a review of cognitive and neuroimaging findings in pathological gambling. Neuroscience & Biobehavioural Reviews, 34, 87–107. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.07.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verdejo-Garcia, A., Lawrence, A. J., & Clark, L. (2008). Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: Review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 777–810. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.11.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., Fowler, J. S., Tomasi, D., Telang, F., & Baler, R. (2010). Addiction: Decreased reward sensitivity and increased expectation sensitivity conspire to overwhelm the brain’s control circuit. BioEssays, 32(9), 748–755. doi:10.1002/bies.201000042.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Walker, M. B. (1992). The psychology of gambling. Oxford: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wardle, H., Moody, A., Spence, S., Orford, J., Volberg, R., Jotangia, D., et al. (2010). British gambling prevalence survey 2010. Birmingham: Gambling Commission.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winstanley, C. A., Eagle, D. M., & Robbins, T. W. (2006). Behavioural models of impulsivity in relation to ADHD: Translation between clinical and preclinical studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 379–395. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.01.001

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, L., Lipszyc, J., Dupuis, A., Sathees, W. T., & Schachar, R. (2014). Response inhibition and psychopathology: A meta-analysis of go no go task performance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(2), 429–439. doi:10.1037/a0036295.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Adrian Parke.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Parke, A., Harris, A., Parke, J. et al. Understanding Within-Session Loss-Chasing: An Experimental Investigation of the Impact of Stake Size on Cognitive Control. J Gambl Stud 32, 721–735 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-015-9570-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-015-9570-x

Keywords

Navigation