Advertisement

Journal of Gambling Studies

, Volume 31, Issue 4, pp 1579–1595 | Cite as

An Expert Map of Gambling Risk Perception

  • Michael Spurrier
  • Alexander Blaszczynski
  • Paul Rhodes
Original Paper

Abstract

The purpose of the current study was to investigate the moderating or mediating role played by risk perception in decision-making, gambling behaviour, and disordered gambling aetiology. Eleven gambling expert clinicians and researchers completed a semi-structured interview derived from mental models and grounded theory methodologies. Expert interview data was used to construct a comprehensive expert mental model ‘map’ detailing risk-perception related factors contributing to harmful or safe gambling. Systematic overlapping processes of data gathering and analysis were used to iteratively extend, saturate, test for exception, and verify concepts and emergent themes. Findings indicated that experts considered idiosyncratic beliefs among gamblers result in overall underestimates of risk and loss, insufficient prioritization of needs, and planning and implementation of risk management strategies. Additional contextual factors influencing use of risk information (reinforcement and learning; mental states, environmental cues, ambivalence; and socio-cultural and biological variables) acted to shape risk perceptions and increase vulnerabilities to harm or disordered gambling. It was concluded that understanding the nature, extent and processes by which risk perception predisposes an individual to maintain gambling despite adverse consequences can guide the content of preventative educational responsible gambling campaigns.

Keywords

Gambling Pathological gambling Harm Risk perception Cognition Decision making Qualitative Grounded theory Mental models 

Notes

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Aarons, G. A., Brown, S. A., Stice, E., & Coe, M. T. (2001). Psychometric evaluation of the marijuana and stimulant effect expectancy questionnaires for adolescents. Addictive Behaviors, 26(2), 219–236.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Andrews, D. A., Bonta, J., & Wormith, J. S. (2006). The recent past and near future of risk and/or need assessment. Crime and Delinquency, 52(1), 7–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Auslander, B. A. (1999). An exploratory study investigating variables in relation to juvenile sexual reoffending. Auslander, USA: The Florida State University.Google Scholar
  4. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Baudinet, J., & Blaszczynski, A. (2012). Arousal and gambling mode preference: A review of the literature. Journal of Gambling Studies, 8(8). doi: 10.1007/s10899-012-9304-2.
  6. Brevers, D., Cleeremans, A., Bechara, A., Laloyaux, C., Kornreich, C., Verbanck, P., et al. (2011). Time course of attentional bias for gambling information in problem gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors,. doi: 10.1037/a0024201.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Chiovitti, R. F., & Piran, N. (2003). Rigour and grounded theory research. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 44(4), 427–435.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Coventry, K., & Norman, A. C. (1998). Arousal, erroneous verbalizations and the illusion of control during a computer-generated gambling task. British Journal of Psychology, 89, 629–645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Delfabbro, P. (2004). The stubborn logic of regular gamblers: Obstacles and dilemmas in cognitive gambling research. Journal of Gambling Studies, 20(1), 1–21.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Delfabbro, P. H., & Winefield, A. H. (1999). Poker machine gambling: An analysis of within session characteristics. British Journal of Psychology, 90, 425–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Derevensky, J., Sklar, A., Gupta, R., & Messerlian, C. (2010). An empirical study examining the impact of gambling advertisements on adolescent gambling attitudes and behaviors. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 8(1), 21–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Fischhoff, B. (1995). Risk perception and communication unplugged: Twenty years of process. Risk Analysis, 15(2), 137–145.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Fortune, E. E., & Goodie, A. S. (2011). Cognitive distortions as a component and treatment focus of pathological gambling: A review. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 26(2), 298–310.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Gillespie, M. A., Derevensky, J., & Gupta, R. (2007). The utility of outcome expectancies in the prediction of adolescent gambling behaviour. Journal of Gambling Issues, 19, 69–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Glanz, K., Rimer, B. K., & Viswanath, K. (2008). Health behavior and health education: Theory, research, and practice (4th ed.). San Francisco, USA: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  16. Goldberg, J., & Fischhoff, B. (2000). The long-term risks in the short-term benefits: Perceptions of potentially addictive activities. Health Psychology, 19(3), 299–303.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Goldstein, R. Z., Craig, A., Bechara, A., Garavan, H., Childress, A. R., & Paulus, M. P. (2009). The neurocircuitry of impaired insight in drug addiction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(9), 372–380.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Griffiths, M. D. (1995). Adolescent gambling. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  19. Hall, H. V., & Poirier, J. G. (2001). Detecting malingering and deception: Forensic distortion analysis (2nd ed.). Boca Raton, USA: CRC Press.Google Scholar
  20. Hayes, N. (1997). Doing qualitative analysis in psychology. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  21. Howard, M., McMillen, C., Nower, L., Elze, D., Edmond, T., & Bricout, J. (2002). Denial in addiction: Towards an integrated stage and process model—qualitative findings. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 34(4), 371–382.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Inglin, S., & Gmel, G. (2011). Beliefs about and attitudes toward gambling in French-speaking Switzerland. Journal of Gambling Studies, 27(2), 299–316.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. James, W. H., Lonczak, H. S., & Moore, D. D. (1996). The role of denial and defensiveness in drug use among adolescents. Journal of Child and Adolescent Substance Abuse, 5(2), 17–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Johansson, A., Grant, J. E., Kim, S. W., Odlaug, B. L., & Gotestam, K. (2009). Risk factors for problematic gambling: A critical literature review. Journal of Gambling Studies, 25(1), 67–92.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Jones, B. T., Corbin, W., & Fromme, K. (2001). A review of expectancy theory and alcohol consumption. Addiction, 96(1), 57–72.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Jungermann, H., Schutz, H., & Thuring, M. (1988). Mental models in risk assessment: Informing people about drugs. Risk Analysis, 8(1), 147–155.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. LaPlante, D. A., Nelson, S. E., LaBrie, R. A., & Shaffer, H. J. (2008). Stability and progression of disordered gambling: Lessons from longitudinal studies. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 53(1), 52–60.Google Scholar
  28. Leigh, B. C. (1999). Peril, chance, adventure: Concepts of risk, alcohol use and risky behavior in young adults. Addiction, 94(3), 371–383.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Lipkus, I. M., Eissenberg, T., Schwartz-Bloom, R. D., Prokhorov, A. V., & Levy, J. (2011). Affecting perceptions of harm and addiction among college waterpipe tobacco smokers. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 13(7), 599–610.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Magura, S., & Kang, S. Y. (1996). Validity of self-reported drug use in high risk populations: A meta-analytical review. Substance Use and Misuse, 31(9), 1131–1153.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Moodie, C. (2007). An exploratory investigation into the erroneous cognitions of pathological and social fruit machine gamblers. Journal of Gambling Issues, 19, 31–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Morgan, M. G., Fischhoff, B., Bostrom, A., & Atman, C. J. (2002). Risk communication: A mental models approach. New York, USA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Nussbaum, D., Honarmand, K., Govoni, R., Kalahani-Bargis, M., Bass, S., Ni, X., et al. (2011). An eight component decision-making model for problem gambling: A systems approach to stimulate integrative research. Journal of Gambling Studies, 27(4), 523–563.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Peretti-Watel, P. (2003). Neutralization theory and the denial of risk: Some evidence from cannabis use among French adolescents. British Journal of Sociology, 54(1), 21–42.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Productivity Commission. (2010). Productivity Commission inquiry report: Gambling. Canberra.Google Scholar
  36. Rachlin, H. (1990). Why do people gamble and keep gambling despite heavy losses? Psychological Science, 1(5), 294–297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Raylu, N., & Oei, T. P. S. (2002). Pathological gambling: A comprehensive review. Clinical Psychology Review, 22(7), 1009–1061.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Rebelo, F. R. (1999). Denial level and coping style in a substance abuse treatment population. Cincinnati, USA: University of Cincinnati.Google Scholar
  39. Richards, L. (2005). Handling qualitative data: A practical guide. London: Sage.Google Scholar
  40. Rinn, W., Desai, N., Rosenblatt, H., & Gastfriend, D. R. (2002). Addiction denial and cognitive dysfunction: A preliminary investigation. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 14(1), 52–57.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B., & Lichtenstein, S. (1978). Accident probabilities and seat belt usage: A psychological perspective. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 10(4), 281–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Smith, G. T., Goldman, M. S., Greenbaum, P. E., & Christiansen, B. A. (1995). Expectancy for social facilitation from drinking: The divergent paths of high-expectancy and low-expectancy adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104(1), 32–40.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Spurrier, M., & Blaszczynski, A. (2013). Risk perception in gambling: A systematic review. Journal of Gambling Studies,. doi: 10.1007/s10899-013-9371-z.Google Scholar
  44. Stanovich, K. E., & West, K. F. (2008). On the relative independence of thinking biases and cognitive ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(4), 672–695.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1994). Grounded theory methodology: An overview. Thousand Oaks, USA: Sage.Google Scholar
  46. Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1998). Basics of qualitative research: Techniques and procedures for developing grounded theory (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, USA: Sage.Google Scholar
  47. Strong, D. R., Breen, R. B., & Lejuez, C. W. (2004). Using item response theory to examine gambling attitudes and beliefs. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1515–1529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Toneatto, T. (1999). Cognitive psychopathology of problem gambling. Substance Use and Misuse, 34(11), 1593–1604.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Toplak, M. E., Liu, E., MacPherson, R., Toneatto, T., & Stanovich, K. E. (2007). The reasoning skills and thinking dispositions of problem gamblers: A dual-process taxonomy. Journal of Behavioural Decision Making, 20, 103–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Wagenaar, W. A. (1988). Paradoxes of gambling behaviour. England: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  51. Walker, M. (1998). The psychology of gambling. Liverpool: Routledge.Google Scholar
  52. Weinstein, N. D. (1987). Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems: Conclusions from a community-wide sample. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 10(5), 481–500.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Weinstein, N. D. (2007). Misleading tests of health behavior theories. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 33(1), 1–10.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Wickwire, E. M., Whelan, J. P., & Meyers, A. W. (2010). Outcome expectancies and gambling behavior among urban adolescents. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 24(1), 75–88.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Wickwire, E. M., Whelan, J. P., West, R., Meyers, A., McCausland, C., & Luellen, J. (2007). Perceived availability, risks, and benefits of gambling among college students. Journal of Gambling Studies, 23(4), 395–408.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Wong, S., & Tsang, S. (2012). Development and validation of the Chinese adolescent gambling expectancy scale. International Gambling Studies, 12(3), 309–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Yi, S., & Kanetkar, V. (2010). Implicit measures of attitudes toward gambling: An exploratory study. Journal of Gambling Issues, 24, 140–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Michael Spurrier
    • 1
  • Alexander Blaszczynski
    • 1
  • Paul Rhodes
    • 1
  1. 1.School of PsychologyUniversity of SydneySydneyAustralia

Personalised recommendations