Journal of Gambling Studies

, Volume 31, Issue 2, pp 409–421 | Cite as

Effect of Casino-Related Sound, Red Light and Pairs on Decision-Making During the Iowa Gambling Task

  • Damien Brevers
  • Xavier Noël
  • Antoine Bechara
  • Nora Vanavermaete
  • Paul Verbanck
  • Charles Kornreich
Original Paper

Abstract

Casino venues are often characterized by “warm” colors, reward-related sounds, and the presence of others. These factors have always been identified as a key factor in energizing gambling. However, few empirical studies have examined their impact on gambling behaviors. Here, we aimed to explore the impact of combined red light and casino-related sounds, with or without the presence of another participant, on gambling-related behaviors. Gambling behavior was estimated with the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT). Eighty non-gamblers participants took part in one of four experimental conditions (20 participants in each condition); (1) IGT without casino-related sound and under normal (white) light (control), (2) IGT with combined casino-related sound and red light (casino alone), (3) IGT with combined casino-related sound, red light and in front of another participant (casino competition—implicit), and (4) IGT with combined casino-related sound, red light and against another participant (casino competition—explicit). Results showed that, in contrast to the control condition, participants in the three “casino” conditions did not exhibit slower deck selection reaction time after losses than after rewards. Moreover, participants in the two “competition” conditions displayed lowered deck selection reaction time after losses and rewards, as compared with the control and the “casino alone” conditions. These findings suggest that casino environment may diminish the time used for reflecting and thinking before acting after losses. These findings are discussed along with the methodological limitations, potential directions for future studies, as well as implications to enhance prevention strategies of abnormal gambling.

Keywords

Decision-making Gambling Environment Sounds Lights Competition 

References

  1. Baumeister, R. F. (1984). Choking under pressure: Self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 610–620.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7–15.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Brand, M., Labudda, K., & Markowitsch, H. J. (2006). Neuropsychological correlates of decision-making in ambiguous and risky situations. Neural Networks, 19, 1266–1276.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. Journal of Personality Social Psychology, 67, 319–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Church, R. M. (1962). The effects of competition on reaction time and palmar skin conductance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65, 32–40.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Cross, J. B., & Gill, D. L. (1982). Competition and instructional set effects on the speed and accuracy of a throwing task. Research Quartely for Exercice and Sport, 53, 125–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Dackis, C., & O’Brien, C. (2005). Neurobiology of addiction: treatment and public policy ramifications. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1431–1436.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Dixon, L., Trigg, R., & Griffiths, M. (2007). An empirical investigation of music and gambling behaviour. International Gambling Studies, 7, 315–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Finlay, K., Kanetkar, V., Londerville, J., & Marmurek, H. H. C. (2006). The physical and psychological measurement of gambling environments. Environment and Behaviour, 38, 570–581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Finlay, K., Marmurek, H. H. C., Kanetkar, V., & Londerville, J. (2007). Trait and state emotion congruence in simulate casinos: Effects on at-risk gambling intention and restoration. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 27, 166–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Finlay, K., Marmurek, H. H. C., Kanetkar, V., & Londerville, J. (2010). Casino Décor effects on gambling emotions and intentions. Environment and Behavior, 42, 524–545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Griffiths, M. D. (1993). Fruit machine gambling: The importance of structural characteristics. Journal of Gambling Studies, 9, 133–152.Google Scholar
  14. Griffiths, M. D., & Auer, M. (2013). The irrelevancy of game-type in the acquisition, development, and maintenance of problem gambling. Frontiers in Psychology. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00621.Google Scholar
  15. Griffiths, M. D., & Parke, J. (2005). The psychology of music in gambling environments: An observational research note. Journal of Gambling Issues, 13. http://www.camh.net/egambling/issue13_jgi_13_griffiths_2html.
  16. Griffiths, M. D., & Swift, G. (1992). The use of light and colour in gambling arcades: A pilot study. Society for the Study of Gambling Newsletter, 21, 16–22.Google Scholar
  17. Hess, H. F., & Diller, J. V. (1969). Motivation for gambling as revealed in the marketing methods of the legitimate gaming industry. Psychological Reports, 25, 19–27.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Houston, J. M., Farese, D. M., & LaDu, T. (1992). Assessing competitiveness: A validation study of the competitiveness index. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 1153–1156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Houston, J. M., Harris, P. B., McIntire, S., & Francis, D. (2002). Revising the competitiveness index. Psychological Reports, 90, 31–34.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Lesieur, H. R., & Blume, S. B. (1987). The South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS): A new instrument for the identification of pathological gamblers. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 1184–1188.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Marmurek, H. H. C., Finlay, K., Kanetkar, V., & Londerville, J. (2007). The influence of music on estimates of at-risk gambling intentions: An analysis by casino design. International Gambling Studies, 7, 113–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Mehrabian, A., & Wixen, W. J. (1986). Preferences for individual video games as a function of their emotional effects on players. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 16, 3–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Milliman, R. E. (1982). Using background music to affect the behaviour of supermarket shoppers. Journal of Marketing, 46, 86–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Peller, A. J., LaPlante, D. A., & Shaffer, H. J. (2008). Parameters for safer gambling behavior: Examining the empirical research. Journal of Gambling Studies, 24, 519–534.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Redish, A. D., Jensen, S., Johnson, A., & Kurth-Nelson, Z. (2007). Reconciling reinforcement learning models with behavioral extinction and renewal: Implications for addiction, relapse, and problem gambling. Psychological Review, 114, 784–805.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Rockloff, M. J., Greer, N., & Fay, C. (2011). The social contagion of gambling: How venue size contributes to player losses. Journal of Gambling Studies, 27, 487–497.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Schultz, W. (2002). Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron, 36, 241–263.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Seta, J. J., Paulus, P. B., & Risner, H. T. (1977). The effects of group composition and evaluation on task performance. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9, 115–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Spenwyn, J., Barrett, D. K., & Griffiths, M. D. (2010). The role of light and music in gambling behaviour: An empirical pilot study. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 8, 107–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Spielberger, C. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: STAI (Eorm I). Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
  31. Stark, G. M., Saunders, D. M., & Wookey, P. E. (1982). Differential effects of red and blue coloured lighting on gambling behaviour. Current Psychological Research, 2, 95–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Ward, J. C., Bitner, M. J., & Barnes, J. (1992). Measuring the prototypicality and measuring of retail environments. Journal of Retailing, 68, 194–220.Google Scholar
  33. Wardle, H., Sproston, K., Orford, J., Erens, B., Griffiths, M., & Constantine, R. (2007). British gambling prevalence survey. London: National Centre for Social Research.Google Scholar
  34. Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). The five factor model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 669–689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Yoto, A., Katsuura, T., Iwanaga, K., & Shimomura, Y. (2007). Effects of object colour stimuli on human brain activities in perception and attention referred to EEG alpha band response. Journal of Physiological Anthropology, 26, 373–379.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Damien Brevers
    • 1
    • 2
  • Xavier Noël
    • 1
  • Antoine Bechara
    • 2
  • Nora Vanavermaete
    • 1
  • Paul Verbanck
    • 1
  • Charles Kornreich
    • 1
  1. 1.Psychological Medicine Laboratory, Faculty of Medicine, Brugmann-campusUniversité Libre de Bruxelles (ULB)BrusselsBelgium
  2. 2.Department of Psychology, Brain and Creativity InstituteUniversity of Southern CaliforniaLos AngelesUSA

Personalised recommendations