Skip to main content
Log in

Expressing Gambling-Related Cognitive Biases in Motor Behaviour: Rolling Dice to Win Prizes

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Gambling Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Cognitive perspectives on gambling propose that biased thinking plays a significant role in sustaining gambling participation and, in vulnerable individuals, gambling problems. One prominent set of cognitive biases include illusions of control involving beliefs that it is possible to influence random gaming events. Sociologists have reported that (some) gamblers believe that it is possible to throw dice in different ways to achieve gaming outcomes (e.g., ‘dice-setting’ in craps). However, experimental demonstrations of these phenomena are lacking. Here, we asked regular gamblers to roll a computer-simulated, but fair, 6 sided die for monetary prizes. Gamblers allowed the die to roll for longer when attempting to win higher value bets, and when attempting to hit high winning numbers. This behaviour was exaggerated in gamblers motivated to keep gambling following the experience of almost-winning in gambling games. These results suggest that gambling cognitive biases find expression in the motor behaviour of rolling dice for monetary prizes, possibly reflecting embodied substrates.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Andres, M., Davare, M., Pesenti, M., Olivier, E., & Seron, X. (2004). Number magnitude and grip aperture interaction. NeuroReport, 15(18), 2773–2777.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benhsain, K., Taillefer, A., & Ladouceur, R. (2004). Awareness of independence of events and erroneous perceptions while gambling. Addictive Behaviors, 29(2), 399–404. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2003.08.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Biner, P. M., Huffman, M. L., Curran, M. A., & Long, K. R. (1998). Illusory control as a function of motivation for a specific outcome in a chance-based situation. Motivation & Emotion, 22(4), 277–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blaszczynski, A., & Nower, L. (2002). A pathways model of problem and pathological gambling. Addiction, 97(5), 487–499. doi:10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00015.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burger, J. M., & Cooper, H. M. (1979). The desirability of control. Motivation and Emotion, 3(4), 381–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chau, A. W., & Phillips, J. G. (1995). Effects of perceived control upon wagering and attributions in computer blackjack. Journal of General Psychology, 122(3), 253–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, D., Sundahl, I., & Lesbo, M. (2000). Illusory personal control as a determinant of bet size and type in casino craps games. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30(6), 1224–1242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 122(3), 371–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, M. R., & Schreiber, J. E. (2004). Near-miss effects on response latencies and win estimations of slot machine players. The Psychological Record, 54, 335–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Domahs, F., Moeller, K., Huber, S., Willmes, K., & Nuerk, H. C. (2010). Embodied numerosity: Implicit hand-based representations influence symbolic number processing across cultures. Cognition, 116(2), 251–266. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, D. S., & Wilson, T. D. (1990). When the stakes are high: A limit to the illusion-of-control effect. Social Cognition, 8(3), 305–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernandez-Duque, D., & Wifall, T. (2007). Actor/observer asymmetry in risky decision making. Judgment and Decision Making Journal, 2(1), 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • First, M., Spitzer, R., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. (2002). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P). New York: Biometrics Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fleming, J. H., & Darley, J. M. (1990). The Purposeful-Action Sequence and the Illusion of Control: The Effects of Foreknowledge and Target Involvement on Observers Judgments of Others Control over Random Events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16(2), 346–357. doi:10.1177/0146167290162014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedland, N., Keinan, G., & Regev, Y. (1992). Controlling the uncontrollable: Effects of stress on illusory perceptions of controllability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(6), 923–931.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golin, S., Terrell, F., & Johnson, B. (1977). Depression and the illusion of control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86(4), 440–442.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, M. D. (1990). The cognitive psychology of gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 6(1), 31–42.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henslin, J. M. (1967). Craps and Magic. The American Journal of Sociology, 73(3), 316–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kassinove, J. I., & Schare, M. L. (2001). Effects of the “near miss” and the “big win” on persistence at slot machine gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15(2), 155–158.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koehler, J. J., Gibbs, B. J., & Hogarth, R. M. (1994). Shattering the illusion of control: Multishot versus single-shot gambles. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 7(3), 183–191. doi:10.1002/bdm.3960070304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253–270.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ladouceur, R., Gaboury, A., Dumont, M., & Rochette, P. (1988). Gambling: Relationship between the frequency of wins and irrational thinking. Journal of Psychology, 122(4), 409–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ladouceur, R., Sylvain, C., Boutin, C., Lachance, S., Doucet, C., Leblond, J., et al. (2001). Cognitive treatment of pathological gambling. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 189(11), 774–780.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(2), 311–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martinez, F., Bonnefon, J.-F., & Hoskens, J. (2009). Active involvement, not illusory control, increases risk taking in a gambling game. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(6), 1063–1071.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, J., & Dickerson, M. (2003). Definition and measurement of chasing in off-course betting and gaming machine play. Journal of Gambling Studies, 19(4), 359–386.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pallesen, S., Mitsem, M., Kvale, G., Johnsen, B. H., & Molde, H. (2005). Outcome of psychological treatments of pathological gambling: A review and meta-analysis. Addiction, 100(10), 1412–1422. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01204.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(2), 110–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raylu, N., & Oei, T. P. (2002). Pathological gambling. A comprehensive review. Clinical Psychology Review, 22(7), 1009–1061.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raylu, N., & Oei, T. P. S. (2004). The Gambling Related Cognitions Scale (GRCS): Development, confirmatory factor validation and psychometric properties. Addiction, 99(6), 757–769. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00753.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reid, R. L. (1986). The psychology of the near miss. Journal of Gambling Behavior, 2, 32–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, P. (1998). The cognitive psychology of lottery gambling: A theoretical review. Journal of Gambling Studies, 14(2), 111–134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steenbergh, T. A., Meyers, A. W., May, R. K., & Whelan, J. P. (2002). Development and validation of the Gamblers’ Beliefs Questionnaire. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16(2), 143–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strickland, L. H., Lewicki, R. J., & Katz, A. M. (1966). Temporal orientation and perceived control as determinants of risk-taking. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2(2), 143–151. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(66)90075-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toneatto, T., Blitz-Miller, T., Calderwood, K., Dragonetti, R., & Tsanos, A. (1997). Cognitive distortions in heavy gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 13(3), 253–266.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 483–488. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wardle, H., Moody, A., Spence, S., Orford, J., Volberg, R., Jotangia, D., et al. (2011). British Gambling Prevalence Survey 2010. London: Stationery Office.

  • Wickwire, E. M., Burke, R. S., Brown, S. A., Parker, J. D., & May, R. K. (2008). Psychometric evaluation of the national opinion research center DSM-IV screen for gambling problems (NODS). American Journal on Addictions, 17(5), 392–395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The research described here was conducted by Matthew S. M. Lim in partial fulfillment of a DPhil research degree for the University of Oxford under the supervision of Robert D. Rogers.

Conflict of interest

None.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert D. Rogers.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOC 689 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lim, M.S.M., Bowden-Jones, H. & Rogers, R.D. Expressing Gambling-Related Cognitive Biases in Motor Behaviour: Rolling Dice to Win Prizes. J Gambl Stud 30, 625–637 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-013-9381-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-013-9381-x

Keywords

Navigation