Journal of Gambling Studies

, Volume 28, Issue 3, pp 393–403 | Cite as

The Effects of Sleep Debt on Risk Perception, Risk Attraction and Betting Behavior During a Blackjack Style Gambling Task

Original Paper

Abstract

Gamblers often gamble while experiencing fatigue due to sleep deprivation or cumulative sleep debt. Such fatigue has been shown to make decision makers behave more riskily. The present study aimed to test the role of two cognitive processes, risk perception and risk attraction, in this effect. Two hundred and two participants played twelve hands of a black-jack style card game while either fatigued or reasonably alert. Findings showed that both fatigued and alert participants rated higher risk bets as more risky than lower risk bets, suggesting risk perception was unaffected by fatigue. However, fatigued participants did not rate higher risk bets as less attractive than lower risk bets, and reduced the size of their wager to a lesser extent when objective risk increased. These findings are discussed in relation to the effects of fatigue on motivated tasks and the need for gamblers to be aware of the effects of fatigue.

Keywords

Gambling Fatigue Risk Sleep Blackjack Betting 

References

  1. Abrams, D., Hopthrow, T., Hulbert, L. G., & Frings, D. (2005). The social context of alcohol consumption: Risk preferences alone and in groups. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 67, 628–636.Google Scholar
  2. Addicott, M. A., & Laurienti, P. J. (2009). A comparison of the effects of caffeine following abstinence and normal caffeine use. Psychopharmacology, 207, 423–431.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Babad, E., & Katz, Y. (1991). Wishful thinking—against all odds. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 21, 1921–1938.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Blascovich, J. (2008). Challenge and threat. In A. J. Elliott (Ed.), Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation (pp. 431–445). Psychology Press: New York.Google Scholar
  5. Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: A meta analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 367–383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Czeisler, C. A., Weitzman, E. D., Moore-Ede, M. C., Zimmerman, J. C., & Kronauer, R. S. (1980). Human sleep: Its duration and organization depend on its circadian phase. Science, 210, 1264–1267.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Frings, D. (2011). The effects of group monitoring on fatigue related Einstellung during mathematical problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Applied. doi:10.1037/a0025131.
  8. Harris, A. J. L., & Hahn, U. (2011). Unrealistic optimism about future life events: A cautionary note. Psychological Review, 118, 135–154.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Harrison, Y., & Horn, J. A. (2000). The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: A review. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6, 236–249.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280–1300.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hopthrow, T., Abrams, D., Frings, D., & Hulbert, L. (2007). Groupdrink: The effects of alcohol on intergroup competitiveness. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 21, 272–276.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Killgore, W. D., Balkin, T. J., & Wesenten, N. J. (2006). Impaired decision making following 49 h of sleep deprivation. Journal of Sleep Research, 15, 7–13.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Krueger, G. P. (1989). Sustained work, fatigue, sleep loss and performance: A review of the issues. Work and Stress, 3, 129–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Lavie, P. (1991). The 24-hour sleep propensity function (SPF): Practical and theoretical implications. In T. H. Monk (Ed.), Sleep, sleepiness and performance (pp. 65–93). Oxford, England: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  15. Li, S., & Liu, C.-J. (2008). Individual differences in a switch from risk-averse preferences for gains to risk-seeking preferences for losses: Can personality variables predict the risk preferences? Journal of Risk Research, 11, 673–686.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Lichtenstein, S., & Slovic, P. (1971). Reversal of preference between bids and choices in gambling decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 46–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. McBride, J., & Derevensky, J. (2009). Internet gambling behavior in a sample of online gamblers. International of Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 7, 149–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. McKenna, B. S., Dickinson, D. L., Orff, H. J., & Drummond, S. P. (2007). The effects of one night of sleep deprivation on known-risk and ambiguous-risk decisions. Journal of Sleep Research, 16, 245–252.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Noy, Y. I., Horrey, W. J., Popkin, S. M., Folkard, S., Howarth, H. D., & Courtney, T. K. (2011). Future directions in fatigue and safety research. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 43, 495–497.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Piper, B. F., Dibble, S. L., Dodd, M. J., Weiss, M. C., Slaughter, R. E., & Paul, S. M. (1998). The revised Piper Fatigue Scale: Psychometric evaluation in women with breast cancer. Oncological Nursing Forum, 25, 677–684.Google Scholar
  21. Rabinbach, A. (1990). The human motor: Energy, fatigue, and the origins of modernity. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  22. Roehrs, T., & Roth, T. (2001). Sleep, sleepiness and alcohol use. Alcohol Research and Health, 25, 101–109.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Roehrs, T., & Roth, T. (2008). Caffeine: Sleep and daytime sleepiness. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 12, 153–162.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Rosenthal, R. J., & Lesieur, H. R. (1992). Self-reported withdrawal symptoms and pathological gambling. The American Journal on Addiction, 1, 150–154.Google Scholar
  25. Steele, C. M., & Josephs, R. A. (1990). Alcohol myopia: Its prized and dangerous effects. American Psychologist, 45, 921–933.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Tversky, A. (1969). Intransitivity of preferences. Psychological Review, 76, 31–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Van der Linden, D., Frese, M., & Meijman, T. F. (2003). Mental fatigue and the control of cognitive processes: Effects on perseveration and planning. Acta Psychologica, 113, 45–65.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Venkatraman, V., Chuah, Y. M. L., Huettel, S. A., & Chee, M. W. L. (2007). Sleep deprivation elevates expectation of gains and attenuates response to losses following risky decisions. Sleep, 30, 603–609.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Weber, E. U., Blais, A.-R., & Betz, N. (2002). A domain-specific risk-attitude scale: Measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of Behavioural Decision Making, 15, 263–290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 806–820.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyLondon South Bank UniversityLondonUK

Personalised recommendations