Advertisement

Journal of Gambling Studies

, Volume 28, Issue 1, pp 123–137 | Cite as

Physiological Responses to Near-Miss Outcomes and Personal Control During Simulated Gambling

  • Luke Clark
  • Ben Crooks
  • Robert Clarke
  • Michael R. F. Aitken
  • Barnaby D. Dunn
Original Paper

Abstract

Near-miss outcomes during gambling are non-win outcomes that fall close to a pay-out. While objectively equivalent to an outright miss, near-misses motivate ongoing play and may therefore be implicated in the development of disordered gambling. Given naturalistic data showing increases in heart rate (HR) and electrodermal activity (EDA) during periods of real gambling play, we sought to explore the phasic impact of win, near-miss and full-miss outcomes on physiological arousal in a controlled laboratory environment. EDA and HR were monitored as healthy, student participants (n = 33) played a simulated slot-machine task involving unpredictable monetary wins. A second gambling distortion, perceived personal control, was manipulated within the same task by allowing the participant to select the play icon on some trials, and having the computer automatically select the play icon on other trials. Near-misses were rated as less pleasant than full-misses. However, on trials that involved personal choice, near-misses produced higher ratings of ‘continue to play’ than full-misses. Winning outcomes were associated with phasic EDA responses that did not vary with personal choice. Compared to full-misses, near-miss outcomes also elicited an EDA increase, which was greater on personal choice trials. Near-misses were also associated with greater HR acceleration than other outcomes. Near-miss outcomes are capable of eliciting phasic changes in physiological arousal consistent with a state of subjective excitement, despite their objective non-win status.

Keywords

Risk-taking Win Loss Arousal Heart rate Skin conductance 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This work was completed within the Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, supported by a consortium award from the MRC and Wellcome Trust (director: TW Robbins). LC received funding from the ESRC and Responsibility in Gambling Trust (RES 164-25-0010) and the British Academy (SG 52374). BD’s involvement in the project was funded by the UK Medical Research Council (U1055.02.002.00001.01). We thank Mr Aaron Louv for assistance with testing. The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

References

  1. Amsel, A. (1992). Frustration theory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson, G., & Brown, R. I. (1984). Real and laboratory gambling, sensation-seeking and arousal. British Journal of Psychology, 75, 401–410.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (1996). Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 215–225.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Boyd, W. H. (1982). Excitement: The gambler’s drug. In W. R. Eadington (Ed.), The gambling papers. Reno, NV: University of Nevada.Google Scholar
  5. Bradley, M. M. (2000). Emotion and motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of Psychophysiology (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  6. Breen, R. B., Kruedelbach, N. G., & Walker, H. I. (2001). Cognitive changes in pathological gamblers following a 28-day inpatient program. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15, 246–248.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brown, R. I. (1986). Arousal and sensation-seeking components in the general explanation of gambling and gambling addictions. International Journal of the Addictions, 21, 1001–1016.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Brown, S. L., Rodda, S., & Phillips, J. G. (2004). Differences between problem and nonproblem gamblers in subjective arousal and affective valence amongst electronic gaming machine players. Addictive Behaviors, 29, 1863–1867.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Camille, N., Coricelli, G., Sallet, J., Pradat-Diehl, P., Duhamel, J. R., & Sirigu, A. (2004). The involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex in the experience of regret. Science, 304, 1167–1170.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cardinal, R. N., & Aitken, M. R. F. (2006). ANOVA for the behavioural sciences researcher. New Jersey, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  11. Chase, H. W., & Clark, L. (2010). Gambling severity predicts midbrain response to near-miss outcomes. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 6180–6187.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Clark, L. (2010). Decision-making during gambling: an integration of cognitive and psychobiological approaches. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 365, 319–330.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Clark, L., Lawrence, A. J., Astley-Jones, F., & Gray, N. (2009). Gambling near-misses enhance motivation to gamble and recruit win-related brain circuitry. Neuron, 61, 481–490.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Cote, D., Caron, A., Aubert, J., Desrochers, V., & Ladouceur, R. (2003). Near wins prolong gambling on a video lottery terminal. Journal of Gambling Studies, 19, 433–438.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Coulombe, A., Ladouceur, R., Desharnais, R., & Jobin, J. (1992). Erroneous perceptions and arousal among regular and occasional video poker players. Journal of Gambling Studies, 8, 235–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Coventry, K. R., & Hudson, J. (2001). Gender differences, physiological arousal and the role of winning in fruit machine gamblers. Addiction, 96, 871–879.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel–now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 59–70.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Crone, E. A., Somsen, R. J., Van Beek, B., & Van Der Molen, M. W. (2004). Heart rate and skin conductance analysis of antecedents and consequences of decision making. Psychophysiology, 41, 531–540.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Cummins, L. F., Nadorff, M. R., & Kelly, A. E. (2009). Winning and positive affect can lead to reckless gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 23, 287–294.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., & Filion, D. L. (2000). The electrodermal system. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of Psychophysiology (2nd ed.). New York, USA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  21. Delfabbro, P., & Winefield, A. H. (1999). Poker-machine gambling: An analysis of within session characteristics. British Journal of Psychology, 90, 425–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Delfabbro, P., & Winefield, A. H. (2000). Predictors of irrational thinking in regular slot machine gamblers. Journal of Psychology, 134, 117–128.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Dickerson, M., & Adcock, S. (1987). Mood, arousal and cognitions in persistent gambling: Preliminary investigation of a theoretical model. Journal of Gambling Studies, 3, 3–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Dixon, M. J., Harrigan, K. A., Sandhu, R., Collins, K., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2010). Losses disguised as wins in modern multi-line video slot machines. Addiction, 105, 1819–1824.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Dixon, M. R., & Schreiber, J. E. (2004). Near-miss effects on response latencies and win estimations of slot machine players. Psychological Record, 54, 335–348.Google Scholar
  26. Fairchild, G., Van Goozen, S. H., Stollery, S. J., & Goodyer, I. M. (2008). Fear conditioning and affective modulation of the startle reflex in male adolescents with early-onset or adolescence-onset conduct disorder and healthy control subjects. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 279–285.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Gaboury, A., & Ladouceur, R. (1989). Erroneous perceptions and gambling. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 4, 411–420.Google Scholar
  28. Goudriaan, A. E., Oosterlaan, J., de Beurs, E., & Van den Brink, W. (2004). Pathological gambling: A comprehensive review of biobehavioral findings. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 28, 123–141.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Griffiths, M. (1991). Psychobiology of the near-miss in fruit machine gambling. Journal of Psychology, 125, 347–357.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Griffiths, M. (1993). Tolerance in gambling: An objective measure using the psychophysiological analysis of male fruit machine gamblers. Addictive Behaviors, 18, 365–372.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Harrigan, K. A. (2008). Slot machine structural characteristics: Creating near misses using high award symbol ratios. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 6, 353–368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Henslin, J. M. (1967). Craps and magic. American Journal of Sociology, 73, 316–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hodes, R. L., Cook, E. W., 3rd, & Lang, P. J. (1985). Individual differences in autonomic response: Conditioned association or conditioned fear? Psychophysiology, 22, 545–560.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kassinove, J. I., & Schare, M. L. (2001). Effects of the “near miss” and the “big win” on persistence at slot machine gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15, 155–158.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Ladouceur, R., & Mayrand, M. (1987). The level of involvement and the timing of betting in roulette. Journal of Psychology, 121, 169–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Ladouceur, R., Sevigny, S., Blaszczynski, A., O’Connor, K., & Lavoie, M. E. (2003). Video lottery: Winning expectancies and arousal. Addiction, 98, 733–738.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Ladouceur, R., & Walker, M. (1996). A cognitive perspective on gambling. In P. M. Salkovskis (Ed.), Trends in cognitive and behavioural therapies. Chichester, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar
  38. Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 311–328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Leary, K., & Dickerson, M. (1985). Levels of arousal in high- and low-frequency gamblers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23, 635–640.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Lesieur, H. R., & Blume, S. B. (1987). The South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS): A new instrument for the identification of pathological gamblers. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 1184–1188.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Lighthall, N. R., Mather, M., & Gorlick, M. A. (2009). Acute stress increases sex differences in risk seeking in the balloon analogue risk task. PLoS ONE, 4, e6002.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Linnet, J., Thomsen, K. R., Moller, A., & Callesen, M. B. (2010). Event frequency, excitement and desire to gamble among pathological gamblers. International Gambling Studies, 10, 177–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Meyer, G., Hauffa, B. P., Schedlowski, M., Pawlak, C., Stadler, M. A., & Exton, M. S. (2000). Casino gambling increases heart rate and salivary cortisol in regular gamblers. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 948–953.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Miller, N. V., & Currie, S. R. (2008). A Canadian population level analysis of the roles of irrational gambling cognitions and risky gambling practices as correlates of gambling intensity and pathological gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 24, 257–274.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Moodie, C., & Finnigan, F. (2005). A comparison of the autonomic arousal of frequent, infrequent and non-gamblers while playing fruit machines. Addiction, 100, 51–59.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Oei, T. P., & Gordon, L. M. (2008). Psychosocial factors related to gambling abstinence and relapse in members of gamblers anonymous. Journal of Gambling Studies, 24, 91–105.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Parke, A., & Griffiths, M. (2004). Gambling addiction and the evolution of the “near miss”. Addiction Research and Therapy, 12, 407–411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Porcelli, A. J., & Delgado, M. R. (2009). Acute stress modulates risk taking in financial decision making. Psychological Science, 20, 278–283.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Raylu, N., & Oei, T. P. (2004). The gambling related cognitions scale (GRCS): Development, confirmatory factor validation and psychometric properties. Addiction, 99, 757–769.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Reid, R. L. (1986). The psychology of the near miss. Journal of Gambling Behaviour, 2, 32–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Rockloff, M. J., Signal, T., & Dyer, V. (2007). Full of sound and fury, signifying something: The impact of autonomic arousal on EGM gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 23, 457–465.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Rushworth, M. F., & Behrens, T. E. (2008). Choice, uncertainty and value in prefrontal and cingulate cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 389–397.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Scholz, U., La Marca, R., Nater, U. M., Aberle, I., Ehlert, U., Hornung, R., et al. (2009). Go no-go performance under psychosocial stress: Beneficial effects of implementation intentions. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 91, 89–92.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Sharpe, L., Tarrier, N., Schotte, D., & Spence, S. H. (1995). The role of autonomic arousal in problem gambling. Addiction, 90, 1529–1540.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Sodano, R., & Wulfert, E. (2010). Cue reactivity in active pathological, abstinent pathological, and regular gamblers. Journal of Gambling Studies, 26, 53–65.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Thompson, S. C., Armstrong, W., & Thomas, C. (1998). Illusions of control, underestimations, and accuracy: A control heuristic explanation. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 143–161.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Toneatto, T., Blitz-Miller, T., Calderwood, K., Dragonetti, R., & Tsanos, A. (1997). Cognitive distortions in heavy gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 13, 253–266.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Venables, P. H., & Mitchell, D. A. (1996). The effects of age, sex and time of testing on skin conductance activity. Biological Psychology, 43, 87–101.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Wilkes, B. L., Gonsalvez, C. J., & Blaszczynski, A. (2010). Capturing SCL and HR changes to win and loss events during gambling on electronic machines. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 78, 265–272.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Wulfert, E., Roland, B. D., Hartley, J., Wang, N., & Franco, C. (2005). Heart rate arousal and excitement in gambling: Winners versus losers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 19, 311–316.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • Luke Clark
    • 1
  • Ben Crooks
    • 1
  • Robert Clarke
    • 1
  • Michael R. F. Aitken
    • 1
  • Barnaby D. Dunn
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of Experimental PsychologyUniversity of CambridgeCambridgeUK
  2. 2.MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences UnitCambridgeUK

Personalised recommendations