Advertisement

Journal of Gambling Studies

, Volume 28, Issue 1, pp 89–103 | Cite as

Executive Function in Pathological Gamblers and Healthy Controls

  • David M. Ledgerwood
  • Emily S. Orr
  • Kristen A. Kaploun
  • Aleks Milosevic
  • G. Ron Frisch
  • Nicholas Rupcich
  • Leslie H. Lundahl
Original Paper

Abstract

Executive function (EF) deficits may underlie some of the impulse control problems seen in pathological gambling. Pathological gamblers (PGs, n = 45) and controls (n = 45) were compared on several measures of EF (including measures of response inhibition, working memory, cognitive flexibility and perseveration, planning and decision-making), as well as memory and intelligence tests to examine whether PGs evidence EF dysfunction. Compared with controls, PGs exhibited specific deficits on measures of planning and decision-making. PGs also exhibited relative deficits on a measure of perseveration, but this deficit was no longer significant after controlling for group differences in intelligence. These results suggest that PGs may experience deficits on specific components of EF.

Keywords

Gambling Pathological gambling Executive function Impulse control Decision-making 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This study and preparation of this report were supported by a grant from the Ontario Problem Gambling Research Centre. The opinions expressed in the publication are not to be construed as those of the Centre or the Ministry of Health and Long-Term Care. We thank Kendrich Bates, Debra Kish, Joi Moore, Lisa Sulkowski, Katie Linehan and the staff of the Windsor Regional Hospital Problem Gambling Services Division.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.), text revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275, 1293–1295.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Berg, E. A. (1948). A simple objective treatment for measuring flexibility in thinking. Journal of General Psychology, 39, 15–22.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Blaszczynski, A., & Steel, Z. (1998). Personality disorders among PGs. Journal of Gambling Studies, 14, 51–71.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brand, M., Kalbe, E., Labudda, K., Fujiwara, E., Kessler, J., & Markowitsch, H. J. (2005). Decision-making impairments in patients with pathological gambling. Psychiatry Research, 133, 91–99.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cavedini, P., Riboldi, G., Keller, R., D’Annucci, A., & Bellodi, L. (2002). Frontal lobe dysfunction in pathological gambling patients. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 334–341.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Dougherty, D. M. (2003). Laboratory behavioral measures of impulsivity: General information manual. Houston, TX: Neurobehavioral Research Laboratory and Clinic, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston.Google Scholar
  8. Ferris, J., & Wynne, H. (2001). The canadian problem gambling index: Final report. Ottawa: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse.Google Scholar
  9. First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1997). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders (SCID-I). Washington: American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
  10. Fischer, S., & Smith, G. T. (2008). Binge eating, problem drinking, and pathological gambling: Linking behaviour to shared traits and social learning. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 789–800.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Gerstein, D., Murphy, S., Toce, M., Hoffmann, J., Palmer, A., Johnson, R., et al. (1999). Gambling impact and behavior study. Chicago: University of Chicago.Google Scholar
  12. Golden, C. J. (1978). Stroop color and word test. Chicago: Stoelting.Google Scholar
  13. Goudriaan, A. E., Oosterlaan, J., de Beurs, E., & van den Brink, W. (2005). Decision making in pathological gambling: A comparison between pathological gamblers, alcohol dependent, persons with Tourette syndrome, and normal controls. Cognitive Brain Research, 23, 137–151.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Goudriaan, A. E., Oosterlaan, J., de Beurs, E., & van den Brink, W. (2006). Neurocognitive functions in pathological gambling: A comparison with alcohol dependence, Tourette syndrome and normal controls. Addiction, 101, 534–547.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Goudriaan, A. E., Oosterlaan, J., de Beurs, E., & van den Brink, W. (2008). The role of self-reported impulsivity and reward sensitivity versus neurocognitive measures of disinhibition and decision-making in the prediction of relapse in pathological gamblers. Psychological Medicine: A Journal of Research in Psychiatry and the Allied Sciences, 38, 41–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hester, R., & Garavan, H. (2004). Executive dysfunction in cocaine addiction: Evidence for discordant frontal, cingulate, and cerebellar activity. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 11017–11022.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hinson, J. M., Jameson, T. L., & Whitney, P. (2003). Impulsive decision making and working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 298–306.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hodgins, D. C. (2004). Using the NORC DSM screen for gambling problems as an outcome measure for pathological gambling: Psychometric evaluation. Addictive Behaviors, 29, 1685–1690.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Lawrence, A. J., Luty, J., Bogdan, N. A., Sahakian, B. J., & Clark, L. (2009). Impulsivity and response inhibition in alcohol dependence and problem gambling. Psychopharmacology, 207, 163–172.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Leblond, J., Ladouceur, R., & Blaszczynski, A. (2003). Which PGs will complete treatment. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42, 205–209.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Ledgerwood, D. M., Alessi, S. M., Phoenix, N., & Petry, N. M. (2009). Behavioral assessment of impulsivity in pathological gamblers with and without a history of substance use disorder versus non-problem gambling controls. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 105, 89–96.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Loring, D. W., Hannay, H. J., & Fischer, J. S. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford.Google Scholar
  23. Milner, B. (1963). Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting. Archives of Neurology, 9, 90–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Petry, N. M. (2001). Substance abuse, pathological gambling, and impulsiveness. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 63, 29–38.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Petry, N. M., Stinson, F. S., & Grant, B. F. (2005). Comorbidity of DSM-IV pathological gambling and other psychiatric disorders: Results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol related conditions. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 66, 564–574.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Potenza, M. N., Leung, H.-C., Blumberg, H. P., Peterson, B. S., Fulbright, R. K., Lacadie, C. M., et al. (2003a). An fMRI Stroop task study of ventromedial prefrontal cortical function in pathological gamblers. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1990–1994.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Potenza, M. N., Steinberg, M. A., Skudlarski, P., Fulbright, R. K., Lacadie, C. M., Wilber, M. K., et al. (2003b). Gambling urges in pathological gambling. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 828–836.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Romer, D., Betancourt, L., Giannetta, J. M., Brodsky, N. L., Farah, M., & Hurt, H. (2009). Executive cognitive functions and impulsivity as correlates of risk taking and problem behaviour in preadolescents. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2916–2926.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 298, 199–209.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Steel, Z., & Blaszczynski, A. (1998). Impulsivity, personality disorders and pathological gambling severity. Addiction, 93, 895–905.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (1998). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms and commentary. New York: Oxford.Google Scholar
  32. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. van Holst, R. J., van den Brink, W., Veltman, D. J., & Goudriaan, A. E. (2010). Why gamblers fail to win: A review of cognitive and neuroimaging findings in pathological gambling. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 87–107.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Vitaro, F., Ferland, F., Jacques, C., & Ladouceur, R. (1998). Gambling, substance use, and impulsivity during adolescence. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 12, 185–194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Voon, V., Thomsen, T., Miyasaki, J. M., de Souza, M., Shafro, A., Fox, S. H., et al. (2007). Factors associated with dopaminergic drug-related pathological gambling in Parkinson disease. Archives of Neurology, 64, 212–216.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • David M. Ledgerwood
    • 1
  • Emily S. Orr
    • 1
  • Kristen A. Kaploun
    • 1
  • Aleks Milosevic
    • 1
  • G. Ron Frisch
    • 2
  • Nicholas Rupcich
    • 3
  • Leslie H. Lundahl
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral NeurosciencesWayne State University School of MedicineDetroitUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of WindsorWindsorCanada
  3. 3.Problem Gambling Services, Windsor Regional HospitalWindsorCanada

Personalised recommendations