Journal of Gambling Studies

, Volume 25, Issue 3, pp 331–342 | Cite as

A Comparison of Ambient Casino Sound and Music: Effects on Dissociation and on Perceptions of Elapsed Time While Playing Slot Machines

Original Paper

Abstract

This research examined the effects of a casino’s auditory character on estimates of elapsed time while gambling. More specifically, this study varied whether the sound heard while gambling was ambient casino sound alone or ambient casino sound accompanied by music. The tempo and volume of both the music and ambient sound were varied to manipulate temporal engagement and introspection. One hundred and sixty (males = 91) individuals played slot machines in groups of 5–8, after which they provided estimates of elapsed time. The findings showed that the typical ambient casino auditive environment, which characterizes the majority of gaming venues, promotes understated estimates of elapsed duration of play. In contrast, when music is introduced into the ambient casino environment, it appears to provide a cue of interval from which players can more accurately reconstruct elapsed duration of play. This is particularly the case when the tempo of the music is slow and the volume is high. Moreover, the confidence with which time estimates are held (as reflected by latency of response) is higher in an auditive environment with music than in an environment that is comprised of ambient casino sounds alone. Implications for casino management are discussed.

Keywords

Gambling Slot machines Ambient casino sound Time perception Dissociation Music 

References

  1. Bailey, N., & Areni, C. S. (2006). When a few minutes sounds like a lifetime: Does atmospheric music expand or contract perceived time? Journal of Retailing, 82(3), 189–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Blaszczynski, A., & Nower, L. (2002). A pathways model of problem and pathological gambling. Addiction, 97, 487–499.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Breen, R. B., & Zimmerman, M. (2002). Rapid onset of pathological gambling in machine gamblers. Journal of Gambling Studies, 18, 31–43.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Cantinotti, M., & Ladouceur, R. (2008). Harm reduction and electronic gambling machines: Does this pair make a happy couple or is divorce foreseen? Journal of Gambling Studies, 24, 39–54.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences (2nd ed.). Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  6. Delfabbro, P. H., & Winefield, A. H. (2000). Predictors of irrational thinking in regular slot machine gamblers. The Journal of Psychology, 13, 117–128.Google Scholar
  7. Diskin, K., & Hodgins, D. C. (1999). Narrowing of attention and dissociation in pathological video lottery gamblers. Journal of Gambling Studies, 15, 17–28.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Dowling, N., Smith, D., & Thomas, T. (2005). Electronic gaming machines: Are they the ‘crack-cocaine’ of gambling? Addiction, 100, 33–45.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Fazio, R. H. (1990). A practical guide to the use of response latency in social psychological research. In C. Hendrick & M. S. Clark (Eds.), Review of personality and social psychology: Vol. 11. Research methods in personality and socialpsychology (pp. 74–97). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  10. Finlay, K., Kanetkar, V., Londerville, J., & Marmurek, H. H. C. (2006). The physical and psychological measurement of gambling environments. Environment and Behavior, 38, 570–581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Finlay, K., Londerville, J., Kanetkar, V., & Marmurek, H. H. C. (2007). Effects of slot machine characteristics on problem gambling behavior. Final report submitted to Ontario Problem Gambling Research Center.Google Scholar
  12. Fisher, S., & Griffiths, M. (1995). Current trends in slot machine gambling: Research and policy issues. Journal of Gambling Studies, 11, 239–247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Griffiths, M. (1999). Gambling technologies: Prospects for problem gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 15, 265–283.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Jacobs, D. F. (1988). Evidence for a common dissociative-like reaction among addicts. Journal of Gambling Behavior, 4(1), 27–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Källmén, H., Andersson, P., & Andren, A. (2008). Are irrational beliefs and depressive mood more common among problem gamblers than non-gamblers? A survey study of Swedish problem gamblers and controls. Journal of Gambling Studies, 24, 441–450.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kaplan, R., & Kaplan, S. (1989). The experience of nature: A psychological perspective. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Kaplan, R., Kaplan, S., & Ryan, R. L. (1998). With people in mind: Design and management of everyday nature. Washington, DC: Island Press.Google Scholar
  18. Kazdin, A. E. (2001). Behavior modification in applied settings. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning.Google Scholar
  19. Kellaris, J. J., & Mantel, S. P. (1996). Shaping time perceptions with background music: The effect of congruity and arousal on estimates of ad duration. Psychology & Marketing, 13(5), 501–516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 311–328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Levitin, D. J. (2006). This is your brain on music. The science of a human obsession. New York: Dutton.Google Scholar
  22. Levitin, D. J., & Menon, V. (2005). The neural locus of temporal structure and expectancies in music: Evidence from functional neuroimaging at 3 tesla. Music Perception, 22(3), 563–575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Lewis, D. J. (1960). Partial reinforcement: A selective review of the literature since 1950. Psychological Bulletin, 57, 1–28.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Lin, C. H., & Wu, S. C. (2006). Influence of audio effects on consumption emotion and temporal perception. Journal of American Academy of Business, 10(1), 174–180.Google Scholar
  25. Marmurek, H. H. C., Finlay, K., Kanetkar, V., & Londerville, J. (2007). The influence of music on estimates of at-risk gambling intentions: An analysis by casino design. International Gambling Studies, 7(1), 113–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. McKinney, M. F., & Moelants, D. (2006). Ambiguity in tempo perception: What draws listeners to different metrical levels? Music Perception, 24(2), 155–166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Parke, J., & Griffiths, M. (2006). The psychology of the fruit machine: The role of structural characteristics (revisited). International Journal of Mental Health Addiction, 4, 151–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Petry, N. M. (2003). A comparison of treatment-seeking pathological gamblers based on preferred gambling activity. Addiction, 98, 645–655.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Turner, N., & Horbay, R. (2004). How do slot machines and other electronic gaming machines actually work? Journal of Gambling Issues, 11, 1–41.Google Scholar
  30. Vines, B. W., Nuzzo, R. L., & Levitin, D. J. (2005). Analyzing temporal dynamics in music: Differential calculus, physics and functional data techniques. Music Perception, 23(2), 137–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Richard Ivey School of BusinessUniversity of Western OntarioLondonCanada
  2. 2.University of GuelphGuelphCanada

Personalised recommendations