Journal of Business and Psychology

, Volume 30, Issue 4, pp 759–772 | Cite as

Examining the Energizing Effects of Humor: The Influence of Humor on Persistence Behavior

Original Paper

Abstract

Purpose

This paper examines whether, when, and how humor can increase individuals’ persistence.

Design/Methodology/Approach

Two laboratory studies were conducted using 124 students from a large Australian university to examine the causal impact of humor exposure on persistence.

Findings

The results show that exposure to humor increases individuals’ persistence in two different tasks and that this effect is mediated by the discrete emotion of amusement (Study 1). Moreover, the positive effect of humor on persistence is stronger for individuals with higher levels of self-enhancing humor style (Study 2).

Implications

Humor is not only entertaining but also replenishing. Individuals engaging in activities that require persistence may benefit from exposure to humor. Therefore, organizations that require their employees to persist may consider creating a playful culture that encourages the use of humor to increase employees’ persistence.

Originality/Value

Our study is the first to systematically examine the influence of humor on persistence. Going beyond anecdotal and correlational evidence, we document the causal impact of humor exposure on persistence using an experimental design. The findings contribute to the psychology of persistence by showing that humor can be used to increase persistence behavior. In addition, ours is the first study to show that the discrete emotion of amusement mediates the relationship between humor and persistence, and that the effect of humor on persistence is greater for individuals who are high in self-enhancing humor style.

Keywords

Persistence Self-regulation Humor Amusement 

References

  1. Abel, M. (2002). Humor, stress and coping strategies. Humor, 15, 365–381. doi:10.1515/humr.15.4.365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Adams, P., & Van Amerongen, J. (1998). House calls: How we can all heal the world one visit at a time. San Francisco: Robert D. Reed Publishers.Google Scholar
  3. Baumeister, R., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego-depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1252.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Bednarski, J. (2012). Eliciting seven discreet positive emotions using film stimuli. (Honors Thesis in Psychology), Vanderbilt University.Google Scholar
  5. Bergkvist, L., & Rossiter, J. (2007). The predictive validity of multiple-item versus single-item measures of the same constructs. Journal of Marketing Research, 44, 175–184. doi:10.1509/jmkr.44.2.175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bergkvist, L., & Rossiter, J. (2009). The importance of choosing one good item for single-item measures and its generalization to all measures. Transfer - Werbeforschung & Praxis, 55(2), 8–18.Google Scholar
  7. Bizi, S., Keinan, G., & Beit-Hallahmi, B. (1988). Humor and coping with stress: A test under real-life conditions. Personality and Individual Differences, 9, 951–956. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(88)90128-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Blau, G. (1993). Operationalizing direction and level of effort and testing the relationships to individual job performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 55, 152–170. doi:10.1006/obhd.1993.1028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Boyle, G., & Joss-Reid, J. (2004). Relationship of humor to health: A psychometric investigation. British Journal of Health Psychology, 9(1), 55–66. doi:10.1348/135910704322778722.
  10. Branson, R. (2007). Losing my virginity: Richard Branson the autobiography. London: Virgin Books.Google Scholar
  11. Ciarocco, N. J., Sommer, K. L., & Baumeister, R. (2001). Ostracism and ego-depletion: The strains of silence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(9), 1156–1163. doi:10.1177/0146167201279008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cogan, R., Cogan, D., Waltz, W., & McCue, M. (1987). Effects of laughter and relaxation on discomfort thresholds. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 10, 139–144. doi:10.1007/BF00846422.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Cooper, C. (2005). Just joking around? Employee humor expression as an ingratiatory behavior. Academy of Management Review, 30(4), 765–775. doi:10.5465/AMR.2005.18378877.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Cooper, C. (2008). Elucidating the bonds of workplace humor: A relational process model. Human Relations, 61(8), 1087–1115. doi:10.1177/0018726708094861.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Crawford, C. (1994). Theory and implications regarding the utilization of strategic humor by leaders. The Journal of Leadership Studies, 1, 53–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Darwin, C. (1965). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  17. DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R., Gailliot, M. T., & Maner, J. K. (2008). Depletion makes the heart grow less helpful: Helping as a function of self-regulatory energy and genetic relatedness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(12), 1653–1662. doi:10.1177/0146167208323981.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Diamantopoulos, A. S., Sarstedt, M., Fuchs, C., Wilczynski, P., & Kaiser, S. (2012). Guidelines for choosing between multi-item and single-item scales for construct measurement: A predictive validity perspective. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 40(3), 434–449. doi:10.1007/s11747-011-0300-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Duncan, J. (1984). Perceived humor and social network patterns in a sample of task-oriented groups: A reexamination of prior research. Human Relations, 37, 895–907.Google Scholar
  20. Ekman, P. (1999). Basic emotions. In T. Dalgleish & M. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion. Sussex: Wiley.Google Scholar
  21. Fessler, D., Pillsworth, E., & Flamson, T. (2004). Angry men and disgusted women: An evolutionary approach to the influence of emotions on risk taking. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 95, 107–123. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.06.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Fleming, P. (2005). Worker’s playtime? Boundaries and cynicism in a “culture of fun” program. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 41(3), 285–303. doi:10.1177/0021886305277033.
  23. Fredrickson, B., & Levenson, R. (1998). Positive emotions speed recovery from the cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 191–220. doi:10.1080/026999398379718.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Fritz, C., & Sonnetag, S. (2006). Recovery, well-being and performance related outcomes: The role of workload and vacation experiences. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 936–945. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.91.4.936.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Gailliot, M. T., Baumeister, R., DeWall, C. N., Maner, J., Plant, E., Tice, D., et al. (2007). Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: Willpower is more than a metaphor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 325–336. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.2.325.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Gibson, D. (1994). Humor consulting: Laughs for power and profit in organizations. Humor, 7, 403–428. doi:10.1515/humr.1994.7.4.403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Gostik, A., & Christopher, S. (2008). The levity effect: Why it pays to lighten up. Hoboken: Wiley.Google Scholar
  28. Grant, A. M. (2007). Relational job design and the motivation to make a prosocial difiference. Academy of Management Review, 32(2), 393–417. doi:10.5465/AMR.2007.24351328.
  29. Greenberg, J. (1987). The college sophomore as guinea pig: Setting the record straight. Academy of Management Review, 17, 157–159. doi:10.2307/258001.Google Scholar
  30. Gross, J., & Levenson, R. (1995). Emotion elicitation using films. Cognition and Emotion, 9(1), 87–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Gruner, C. (1997). The game of humor: A comprehensive theory of why we laugh. New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
  32. Hagger, M., Wood, C., Stiff, C., & Chatzisarantis, N. (2010). Ego-depletion and the strength model of self control: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 496–525. doi:10.1037/a0019486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hamilton, R., Vohs, K., Sellier, A., & Meyvis, T. (2011). Being of two minds: Switching mindsets exhausts self-regulatory resources. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 115(1), 13–24. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2010.11.005.
  34. Henman, L. (2001). Humor as a coping mechanism: Lessons from POWs. Humor, 14, 83–94. doi:10.1515/humr.14.1.83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Herring, D., Burleson, M., Roberts, N., & Devine, M. (2011). Coherent with laughter: Subjective experience, behavior, and physiological responses during amusement and joy. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 79, 211–218. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.10.007.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Highhouse, Scott. (2009). Designing experiments that generalize. Organizational Research Methods, 12(3), 554–566. doi:10.1177/1094428107300396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Hobbes, T. (1651). Leviathan. Middlesex: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
  38. Holmes, J., & Marra, M. (2002). Having a laugh at work: How humour contributes to workplace culture. Journal of Pragmatics, 34, 1683–1709. doi:10.1016/S0378-2166(02)00032-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Izard, C. (1993). Organizational and motivational functions of discrete emotions. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of Emotions (pp. 631–641). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  40. Keltner, D. (2008). Born to be good: The science of a meaningful life. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
  41. Keltner, D., & Bonanno, G. (1997). A study of laughter and dissociation: Distinct correlates of laughter and smiling during bereavement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(4), 687–702. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.4.687.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Korczynski, M. (2011). The dialectical sense of humor: Routine joking in a taylorized factory. Organization Studies, 32(10), 1421–1439. doi:10.1177/0170840611421256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Kuiper, N., & Martin, R. (1998). Laughter and stress in daily life: Relation to positive and negative affect. Motivation and Emotion, 22, 1133–1153. doi:10.1023/A:1021392305352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Kuiper, N., & McHale, N. (2009). Humor styles as mediators between self-evaluative standards and psychological well-being. The Journal of Psychology, 143(4), 359–376. doi:10.3200/JRLP.143.4.359-376.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Lehmann-Willenbrock, N., & Allen, J. A. (2014). How fun are your meetings? Investigating the relationship between humor patterns in team interactions and team performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99(6), 1278–1287. doi:10.1037/a0038083.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Lindebaum, D., & Jordan, P. (2012). Positive emotions, negative emotions, or utility of discrete emotions. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 33, 1027–1030. doi:10.1002/job.1819.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Lockwood, N., & Yoshimura, S. (2013). The heart of the matter: The effects of humor on well-being during recovery from cardiovascular disease. Health Communication,. doi:10.1080/10410236.2012.762748.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Malone, P. (1980). Humor: A double-edge tool for today’s managers? Academy of Management Review, 5, 4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Martin, R. (1998). Approaches to sense of humor: A historical review. In W. Ruch (Ed.), The sense of humor: Explorations of a personality characteristic (pp. 15–60). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Martin, R. (2007). The psychology of humor. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  51. Martin, R., & Lefcourt, H. (1983). Sense of humor as a moderator of the relationship between stressors and moods. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 1313–1324. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.45.6.1313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Martin, R., Puhlik-Doris, P., Larsen, G., Gray, J., & Weir, K. (2003). Individual differences in uses of humor and their relation to psychological well-being: Development of the humor styles questionnaire. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 48–75. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00534-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Milkman, K. (2012). Unsure what the future will bring? You may overindulge: Uncertainty increases the appeal of wants over shoulds. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 119, 163–176. doi:10.2139/ssrn.1393535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Mindess, H. (1971). Laughter and liberation. Los Angeles: Nash Publishing.Google Scholar
  55. Morreall, J. (1983). Taking laughter seriously. Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
  56. Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. (2000). Self regulation and the depletion of limited resources: Does self control resemble a muscle. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247–259. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.126.2.247.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Muraven, M., Tice, D., & Baumeister, R. (1998). Self-control as limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 774–789. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.126.2.247.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Nezlek, J., & Derks, P. (2001). Use of humor as a coping mechanism, psychological adjustment and social interaction. Humor, 14, 395–413. doi:10.1515/humr.2001.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Oaten, M., & Cheng, K. (2006). Longitudinal gains in self-regulation from regular physical exercise. British Journal of Health Psychology, 11, 717–733. doi:10.1348/135910706X96481.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Plester, B. (2007). “Taking the piss”: Functions of banter in the IT Industry. Humor, 20(2), 152–187. doi:10.1515/HUMOR.2007.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Plester, B. (2009). Crossing the line: Boundaries of workplace humour and fun. Employee Relations, 31(6), 584–599. doi:10.1108/01425450910991749.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Quinn, R., Spreitzer, G., & Lam, C. (2012). Building a sustainable model of human energy in organizations: Exploring the critical role of resources. The Academy of Management Annals, 6(1), 337–396. doi:10.1080/19416520.2012.676762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Robert, C., & Wilbanks, J. E. (2012). The Wheel Model of humor: Humor events and affect in organizations. Human Relations, 65(9), 1071–1099. doi:10.1177/0018726711433133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Romero, E., & Cruthirds, K. (2006). The use of humor in the workplace. The Academy of Management Perspectives, 20, 58–69. doi:10.5465/AMP.2006.20591005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Romero, E., & Pescosolido, A. (2008). Humor and group effectiveness. Human Relations, 61, 395–418. doi:10.1177/0018726708088999.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Rossiter, J. (2002). The C-OAR-SE procedure for scale development in marketing. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 19(4), 305–335. doi:10.1016/S0167-8116(02)00097-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Rottenberg, J., Kasch, K., Gross, J. J., & Gotlib, I. (2002). Sadness and amusement reactivity differentially predict concurrent and prospective functioning in major depressive disorder. Emotion, 2, 135–146.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Rottenberg, J., Ray, D., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Emotion elicitation using films. In J. Coan & J. Allen (Eds.), The handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  69. Sandelands, L., Brockner, J., & Glynn, M. (1988). If at first you don’t succeed, try, try and try again: Effects of persistence-performance contingencies, ego involvement and self esteem on task persistence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 208–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Sauter, D., & Scott, S. (2005). More than one kind of happiness: Can we recognize vocal expressions of different positive states? Motivation and Emotion, 31, 192–199. doi:10.1007/s11031-007-9065-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Shiota, M., Campos, R., & Keltner, D. (2003). The faces of positive emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1000, 296–299. doi:10.1196/annals.1280.029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Shiota, M., Keltner, D., & John, O. (2006). Positive emotional dispositions differentially associated with big five personality and attachment style. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 1, 61–71. doi:10.1080/17439760500510833.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Strick, M., Holland, R. W., van Baaren, R. B., & van Knippenberg, A. (2009). Finding comfort in a joke: Consolatory effects of humor through cognitive distraction. Emotion, 9(4), 574–578. doi:10.1037/a0015951.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Tangney, June P., Baumeister, Roy F., & Boone, Angie L. (2004). High self control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(2), 270–322. doi:10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x/.Google Scholar
  75. Tews, M., Michel, J., & Stafford, K. (2013). Does fun pay? The impact of workplace fun on employee turnover and performance. Cornell Hospitality Quarterly, 54(4), 370–382. doi:10.1177/1938965513505355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Thoman, D., Smith, J., & Silvia, P. (2011). The resource replenishment function of interest. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2, 592–599. doi:10.1177/1948550611402521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Tice, D. M., Baumeister, R., Shmueli, D., & Muraven, M. (2007). Restoring the self: Positive affect helps improve self-regulation following ego-depletion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 379–384. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.05.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Tiedens, L. (2001). The Effect of Anger on the Hostile Inferences of Aggressive and Nonaggressive People: Specific Emotions, Cognitive Processing, and Chronic Accessibility. Motivation and Emotion, 25(3), 233-251. doi: 10.1023/A:1012224507488.
  79. Vaillant, G. (2000). Adaptive mental mechanisms: Their role in a positive psychology. American Psychologist, 55(1), 89–98. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.89.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Vohs, K., Baumeister, R., & Ciarocco, N. J. (2005). Self-regulation and self-presentation: regulatory resource depletion impairs impression management and effortful self-presentation depletes regulatory resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(4), 632–657. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.4.632.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Vohs, K., Baumeister, R., Schmeichel, B. J., Twenge, J. M., Nelson, N. M., & Tice, D. (2008). Making choices impairs subsequent self-control: A limited-resource account of decision making, self-regulation, and active initiative. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 883–898. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.883.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Warren, S., & Fineman, S. (2007). Don’t get me wrong it’s fun here but. In R. Westwood, C. Rhodes, et al. (Eds.), Humour, work and organization. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  83. Wegner, D., Schneider, D., Carter, S., & White, T. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 5–13. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.53.1.5.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  84. Weisenberg, M., Tepper, L., & Schwarzwald, J. (1995). Humor as a coping technique for increasing pain tolerance. Pain, 63, 207–212. doi:10.1016/0304-3959(95)00046-U.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  85. Williams, L., & Destano, D. (2008). Pride and Perserverance: The motivational role of pride. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(6), 1007–1017. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.6.1007.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. Wyer, R., & Collins, J. (1992). A theory of humor elicitation. Psychological Review, 4, 663–688.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Wyland, C., & Forgas, J. (2007). On bad mood and white bears: The effects of mood state on ability to suppress unwanted thoughts. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 1513–1524. doi:10.1080/02699930601063506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Yovetich, N., Dale, J., & Hudak, M. (1990). Benefits of humor in reduction of threat induced anxiety. Psychological Reports, 66, 51–58. doi:10.2466/pr0.1990.66.1.51.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  89. Zhou, X., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. (2009). The symbolic power of money: Reminders of money alter social distress and physical pain. Psychological Science, 20(6), 700–706. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02353.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  90. Zillman, D., Rockwell, S., Schweitzer, K., & Sundar, S. (1993). Does humor facilitate coping with physical discomfort? Motivation and Emotion, 17, 1–21. doi:10.1007/BF00995204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Zweyer, K., Velker, B., & Ruch, W. (2004). Do cheerfulness, exhilaration and humor production moderate pain tolerance? A FACS study. Humor, 17(1), 85–119.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Management, Australian School of BusinessUniversity of New South WalesSydneyAustralia

Personalised recommendations