Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Working memory moderates the association between condom use intentions and behavior among moderate-to-heavy drinking men who have sex with men

  • Brief Report
  • Published:
Journal of Behavioral Medicine Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Men who have sex with men (MSM) are a high-risk population for HIV infection and this risk is increased for those who consume alcohol. Condomless anal intercourse (CAI) is the central transmission risk factor for this population. This study examined whether individual differences in working memory moderated the association between intentions to use condoms and the frequency of CAI among MSM who engaged in anal intercourse over a subsequent 6-week period. Moderate- and heavy-drinking MSM (n = 207) completed questionnaires regarding alcohol use and condom use intentions and an operation span task to assess working memory at baseline. Participants then completed 6 weeks of morning surveys via a mobile phone app to assess anal intercourse frequency with and without condoms. Negative binomial regression analyses showed that the association between intentions to use condoms and episodes of CAI during the monitoring period was moderated by working memory such that intentions predicted CAI for those high in working memory but not those low in working memory. These results support the view that self-reported intentions may be less-likely to translate into health behaviors among those with poorer executive functioning skills.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Notes

  1. Three participants had morning survey scores that exceeded the maximum range (due to data or procedural irregularities) and were therefore not included in analyses.

References

  • Ajzen, I., & Madden, T. J. (1986). Prediction of goal-directed behavior: Attitudes, intentions, and perceived behavioral control. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,22, 453–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albarracín, D., Durantini, M. R., & Earl, A. (2006). Empirical and theoretical conclusions of an analysis of outcomes of HIV-prevention interventions. Current Directions in Psychological Science,15, 73–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allan, J. L., Johnston, M., & Campbell, N. (2011). Missed by an inch or a mile? Predicting the size of intention—behavior gap from measures of executive control. Psychology & Health,26, 635–650.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andrew, B. J., Mullan, B. A., de Wit, J. B., Monds, L. A., Todd, J., & Kothe, E. J. (2016). Does the theory of planned behaviour explain condom use behaviour among men who have sex with men? A meta-analytic review of the literature. AIDS and Behavior,20, 2834–2844.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F., Tugade, M. M., & Engle, R. W. (2004). Individual differences in working memory capacity and dual-process theories of the mind. Psychological Bulletin,130, 553573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bickel, W. K., Yi, R., Landes, R. D., Hill, P. F., & Baxter, C. (2011). Remember the future: Working memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts. Biological Psychiatry,69, 260–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cahalan, D., Cisin, I. H., & Crossley, H. M. (1969). American drinking practices: A national study of drinking behavior and attitudes. Monographs of the Rutgers Center of Alcohol Studies, 6, 260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, M. L. (2010). Toward a person × situation model of sexual risk-taking behaviors: Illuminating the conditional effects of traits across sexual situations and relationship contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,98, 319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finn, P. R., Justus, A., Mazas, C., & Steinmetz, J. E. (1999). Working memory, executive processes and the effects of alcohol on Go/No-Go learning: Testing a model of behavioral regulation and impulsivity. Psychopharmacology (Berl),146, 465–472.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, J. D., & Fisher, W. A. (2002). The information-motivation-behavioral skills model. Emerging theories in health promotion practice and research: Strategies for improving public health,1, 40–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, J. D., Fisher, W. A., Bryan, A. D., & Misovich, S. J. (2002). Information-motivation-behavioral skills model-based HIV risk behavior change intervention for inner-city high school youth. Health Psychology,21, 177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A metaanalysis of effects and processes. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 38, pp. 69–119). San Diego: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A. (1982). Précis of The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,5, 469–484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirsh, J. B., Galinsky, A. D., & Zhong, C. B. (2011). Drunk, powerful, and in the dark: How general processes of disinhibition produce both prosocial and antisocial behavior. Perspectives on Psychological Science,6, 415–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., Baumeister, R. F., Förster, G., & Vohs, K. D. (2012a). Everyday temptations: An experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,102, 1318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., Friese, M., & Wiers, R. W. (2008a). Impulsive versus reflective influences on health behavior: A theoretical framework and empirical review. Health Psychology Review,2, 111–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Friese, M., Wiers, R. W., & Schmitt, M. (2008b). Working memory capacity and self-regulatory behavior: Toward an individual differences perspective on behavior determination by automatic versus controlled processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,95, 962.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2012b). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,16, 174–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hogarth, L., Attwood, A. S., Bate, H. A., & Munafò, M. R. (2012). Acute alcohol impairs human goal-directed action. Biological Psychology,90, 154–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Houben, K., Dassen, F. C., & Jansen, A. (2016). Taking control: Working memory training in overweight individuals increases self-regulation of food intake. Appetite,105, 567–574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Houben, K., Wiers, R. W., & Jansen, A. (2011). Getting a grip on drinking behavior: Training working memory to reduce alcohol abuse. Psychological Science,22, 968–975.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in cognitive sciences,14, 317–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koblin, B. A., Husnik, M. J., Colfax, G., Huang, Y., Madison, M., Mayer, K., et al. (2006). Risk factors for HIV infection among men who have sex with men. Aids,20, 731–739.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maisto, S. A., Palfai, T., Vanable, P. A., Heath, J., & Woolf-King, S. E. (2012). The effects of alcohol and sexual arousal on determinants of sexual risk in men who have sex with men. Archives of Sexual Behavior,41, 971–986.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maisto, S. A., & Simons, J. S. (2016). Research on the effects of alcohol and sexual arousal on sexual risk in men who have sex with men: Implications for HIV prevention interventions. AIDS and Behavior,20, 158–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marteau, T. M., Hollands, G. J., & Fletcher, P. C. (2012). Changing human behavior to prevent disease: The importance of targeting automatic processes. Science,337, 1492–1495.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McEachan, R. R. C., Conner, M., Taylor, N. J., & Lawton, R. J. (2011). Prospective prediction of health-related behaviours with the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology Review,5, 97–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McFarland, W., Chen, Y. H., Nguyen, B., Grasso, M., Levine, D., Stall, R., et al. (2012). Behavior, intention or chance? A longitudinal study of HIV seroadaptive behaviors, abstinence and condom use. AIDS and Behavior,16, 121–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montanaro, E. A., & Bryan, A. D. (2014). Comparing theory-based condom interventions: Health belief model versus theory of planned behavior. Health Psychology,33, 1251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheeran, P., & Webb, T. L. (2016). The intention–behavior gap. Social and Personality Psychology Compass,10, 503–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shuper, P. A., Joharchi, N., Monti, P. M., Loutfy, M., & Rehm, J. (2017). Acute alcohol consumption directly increases HIV transmission risk: A randomized controlled experiment. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes, 76(5), 493–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teng, Y., & Mak, W. W. (2011). The role of planning and self-efficacy in condom use among men who have sex with men: An application of the health action process approach model. Health Psychology,30, 119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language,28, 127–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychological Bulletin,132, 249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wray, T. B., Celio, M. A., Pérez, A. E., DiGuiseppi, G. T., Carr, D. J., Woods, L. A., & Monti, P. M. (2019). Causal effects of alcohol intoxication on sexual risk intentions and condom negotiation skills among high-risk men who have sex with men (MSM). AIDS and Behavior, 23(1), 161–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This study was supported by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism of the National Institute of Health under the award number R01AA022301.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to K. D. Tahaney.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

Kelli Tahaney, Tibor Palfai, Peter Luehring-Jones, Stephen Maisto, and Jeffrey Simons declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all patients for being included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tahaney, K.D., Palfai, T.P., Luehring-Jones, P. et al. Working memory moderates the association between condom use intentions and behavior among moderate-to-heavy drinking men who have sex with men. J Behav Med 43, 666–671 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-019-00090-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-019-00090-9

Navigation