Skip to main content
Log in

Identifying Community-Based Reinforcers of Adults with Autism and Related Disabilities

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Behavioral Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A forced-choice preference assessment using pictures and no access upon selection was used to determine preferences for community-based activities with 4 young adults with autism and intellectual disability. High- and low-preference activities were then provided as delayed consequences, using a token economy, for completion of vocational tasks in a concurrent operants paradigm. All participants responded to the contingency associated with earning the high-preference activity and away from the contingency associated with earning the low-preference activity. The results suggest that a pictorial assessment without access is a valid method for identifying community-based activities that will function as reinforcers. This efficient protocol could improve treatment efficacy in applied settings.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Clevenger, T. M., & Graff, R. B. (2005). Assessing object-to-picture and picture-to-object matching as prerequisite skills for pictorial preference assessments. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 543–547.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen-Almeida, D., Graff, R. B., & Ahearn, W. H. (2000). A comparison of verbal and tangible stimulus preference assessments. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 329–334.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Conyers, C., Doole, A., Vause, T., Harapiak, S., Yu, D. C. T., & Martin, G. L. (2002). Predicting the relative efficacy of three presentation methods for assessing preferences of persons with developmental disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 49–58.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • DeLeon, I. G., Frank, M. A., & Gregory, M. K. (2009). On the correspondence between preference assessment outcomes and progressive-ratio schedule assessments of stimulus value. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42(3), 729–733.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian, L. P., Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A comparison of two approaches for identifying reinforcers for persons with severe and profound disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 491–498.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Graff, R. B., Gibson, L., & Galiatsatos, T. G. (2006). The impact of high- and low-preference stimuli on vocational and academic performances of youths with severe disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 131–135.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Green, C. W., Reid, D. H., Canipe, V. S., & Gardner, S. M. (1991). A comprehensive evaluation of reinforcer identification processes for persons with profound multiple handicaps. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 537–552.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Groskreutz, M. P., & Graff, R. B. (2009). Evaluating pictorial preference assessment: The effect of differential outcomes on preference assessment results. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 113–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hanley, G. P. (2010). Toward effective and preferred programming: A case for objective measurement of social validity with recipients of behavior-change programs. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 3, 13–21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & Lindberg, J. S. (1999). Analysis of activity preferences as a function of differential consequences. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 419–435.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, M. S. H., Yu, C. T., Martin, T. L., & Martin, G. L. (2010). On the relation between reinforcer efficacy and preference. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43(1), 95–100.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Northup, J., George, T., Jones, K., Broussard, C., & Vollmer, T. R. (1996). A comparison of reinforcer assessment methods: The utility of verbal and pictorial choice procedures. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 201–212.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Pace, G. M., Ivancic, M. T., Edwards, G. L., Iwata, B. A., & Page, T. J. (1985). Assessment of stimulus preference and reinforcer value with profoundly retarded individuals. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 249–255.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Reid, D. H., Parsons, M. B., & Green, C. W. (1998). Identifying work preferences among individuals with severe multiple disabilities prior to beginning supported work. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 281–285.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tessing, J. L., Napolitano, D. A., McAdam, D. B., DiCesare, A., & Axelrod, S. (2006). The effects of providing access to stimuli following choice making during vocal preference assessments. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 501–506.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Ashley Williams, Melissa Kelly, Izumi Nishida, and Lisa Goldthwaite for their assistance with data collection and interobserver agreement data. This research was based on a doctoral dissertation by the first author toward completion of a Ph.D. degree in Applied Behavior Analysis.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniel A. Almeida.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

Daniel Almeida declares that he has no conflict of interest. Ronald Allen declares that he has no conflict of interest. Russell Maguire declares he has no conflict of interest. Kaitlin Maguire declares she has no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Appendix

Appendix

Procedural steps for each participant during the forced-choice preference assessment and during the baseline and concurrent operants reinforcer assessment conditions of the reinforcer assessment were assessed for treatment integrity.

Forced-Choice Preference Assessment: All Participants

  1. 1.

    Two photographs were placed side by side on table top according to data sheet.

  2. 2.

    Experimenter told participants to “pick your favorite” prior to trial one and prior to trials 10 and 20.

  3. 3.

    When a photograph was touched by a participant, the experimenter ended the trial and both photographs were removed.

Reinforcer Assessment: Baseline

William

  1. 1.

    The experimenter held a blue and orange utensil in each hand at chest height.

  2. 2.

    When William approached the experimenter within four feet, the experimenter said, “match.”

  3. 3.

    After William took a utensil, the experimenter held up two more utensils (see #1).

Fred

  1. 1.

    The experimenter placed two plastic three inch x five inch file boxes and two stacks of index cards, one orange and the other blue, with names (i.e., last name, first name) written across the top of each card, in front of the participant.

  2. 2.

    The experimenter cued “time to file.”

    (There were no programmed consequences for either accurate or inaccurate filing.)

Charles and Karl

  1. 1.

    The experimenter placed two identical stacks of paper in front of the participant.

  2. 2.

    The experimenter cued “time to shred.”

    (There were no programmed consequences for shredding paper from either stack of paper.)

Reinforcer Assessment: Concurrent Operants Condition

William

Procedures were similar to those in baseline except for the following:

  1. 1.

    The experimenter placed two 20 token boards on the table in front of the six bins.

  2. 2.

    The experimenter placed a picture of the community activity associated with completion of that task next to each token board.

  3. 3.

    The experimenter said, “time to sort, you can earn (highest preferred community activity) for sorting the orange utensils, you can earn (least preferred community activity) for sorting the blue utensils.”

  4. 4.

    The experimenter delivered tokens on a FR3 schedule for accurate sorting.

  5. 5.

    If the token board was not filled by the conclusion of a session, the experimenter said, “You can earn more tokens in a few minutes.”

  6. 6.

    When 20 tokens were earned, the experimenter delivered a coupon representing the community activity associated with that token board.

Fred

During the concurrent operants phase, procedures were similar to those in baseline except for the following:

  1. 1.

    The experimenter placed two 20 token boards on the table.

  2. 2.

    The experimenter placed next to each token board a picture of the community activity associated with completion of that task.

  3. 3.

    The experimenter said, “time to file, you can earn (highest preferred community activity) for doing task one, you can earn (least preferred community activity) for doing task two.”

  4. 4.

    The experimenter delivered tokens on a CRF schedule for the first three token deliveries, then on a FR2 schedule for the next three token deliveries, and then on a FR3 schedule for accurate filing for the remainder of the five minute sessions.

  5. 5.

    If the token board was not filled by the conclusion of a session, the experimenter said, “You can earn more tokens in a few minutes.”

  6. 6.

    When 20 tokens were earned, the experimenter delivered a coupon representing the community activity associated with that token board.

Charles and Karl

During the concurrent operants phase, procedures were similar to those in baseline except for the following:

  1. 1.

    The experimenter placed two 20 token boards, one black and the other white, on the table in front of the participant.

  2. 2.

    The experimenter placed next to each token board a picture of the community activity associated with completion of that task.

  3. 3.

    The experimenter said, “time to shred, you can earn (highest preferred community activity) for doing task one, you can earn (least preferred community activity) for doing task two.”

  4. 4.

    The experimenter delivered tokens on a FR5 schedule for Charles and on a FR3 schedule for Karl for shredding paper from either stack.

  5. 5.

    If the token board was not filled by the conclusion of a session, the experimenter said, “You can earn more tokens in a few minutes.”

  6. 6.

    When 20 tokens were earned, the experimenter delivered a coupon representing the community activity associated with that token board.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Almeida, D.A., Allen, R., Maguire, R.W. et al. Identifying Community-Based Reinforcers of Adults with Autism and Related Disabilities. J Behav Educ 27, 375–394 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-018-9295-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-018-9295-x

Keywords

Navigation