Skip to main content
Log in

Sexual Attitudes and Instructional Set Affect Estimates of Risk and Response Effectiveness

  • Published:
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two hundred and eleven undergraduate men and women were assigned to different instructional set conditions and asked to rate (1) the sexual risk depicted in a set of written items describing problem situations undergraduate women might face when dating or interacting socially with men, and (2) the effectiveness of responses to these situations, described by a set of response codes. Results revealed that sexual attitudes and instructional set were the strongest predictors of both ratings. Gender did, however, predict risk ratings for stranger and authority figure situations, with women rating these situations as more risky than men. In addition, women assigned to the risk instructional set condition rated refusal responses as more effective in decreasing risk than did men in the same condition. In contrast, women assigned to the popularity instructional set condition rated the same refusal responses as less effective in increasing popularity than did men in that condition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Further information regarding the development of the items is available from the first author.

  2. The items were constructed to be nonoverlapping and independent. However, in the present study, these content categories were used to assess whether participants’ ratings of the items and possible responses to them would vary as a function of the particular type of problem depicted.

  3. Further information regarding the development of the response codes is available from the first author.

  4. The popularity dimension ratings were of key importance in the past research that focused on the development and evaluation of the items. The analyses regarding this dimension will not be presented in the current study.

REFERENCES

  • Agostinelli, G., & Miller, W. R. (1994). Drinking and thinking: How does personal drinking affect judgments of prevalence and risk? Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 55, 327–337.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Abbey, A. (1982). Sex differences in attributions for friendly behavior: Do males misperceive females’ friendliness? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 830–838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abbey, A., & Melby, C. (1986). The effects of nonverbal cues on gender differences in perceptions of sexual intent. Sex Roles, 15, 283–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abbey, A., Cozzarelli, C., McLaughlin, K., & Harnish, R. J. (1987). The effects of clothing and dyad sex composition on perceptions of sexual intent: Do women and men evaluate these cues differently? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 17, 108–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, M. J., & Kirk, K. M. (2000). Do individual differences in sociosexuality represent genetic or environmentally contingent strategies? Evidence from the Australian Twin Registry. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 537–545.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, N. J., O’Neal, K. K., Feng, D., & Schoenrock, C. J. (1999). Gender and sexual risk. Sex Roles, 41, 313–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cue, K. L., George, W. H., & Norris, J. (1996). Women's appraisals of sexual-assault risk in dating situations. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20, 487–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeCoster, J., & Claypool, H. M. (2004). A meta-analysis of priming effects on impression formation supporting a general model of informational biases. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 2–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1976). Sex and personality. Austin: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finucane, M. L., Slovic, P., Mertz, C. K., Flynn, J., & Satterfield, T. A. (2000). Gender, race, and perceived risk: The ‘white male’ effect. Health, Risk, & Society, 2, 159–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flynn, J., Slovic, P., & Mertz, C. K. (1994). Gender, race, and perception of environmental health risks. Risk Analysis, 14, 1101–1108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gerrard, M., Gibbons, F. X., Benthin, A. C., & Hessling, R. M. (1996). A longitudinal study of the reciprocal nature of risk behaviors and cognitions in adolescents: What you do shapes what you think, and visa versa. Health Psychology, 15, 344–354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, A. P., Carr, E. G., Davidson, W. S., & Wehr, P. (1981). Response to aggression: Methods of control and prosocial alternatives. New York: Permagon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harnish, R. J., Abbey, A., & DeBono, K. G. (1990). Toward an understanding of the “sex game”: The effects of gender and self-monitoring on perceptions of sexuality and likability in initial interactions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 20, 1333–1344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoecker, K. S., & White, J. W. (1995). Perceptions of invulnerability to sexual assault. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, New York.

  • Kirchner, E. P., Kennedy, R. E., & Draguns, J. G. (1979). Assertion and aggression in adult offenders. Behavior Therapy, 10, 452–471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koss, M. P., & Gidycz, C. A. (1985). Sexual Experiences Survey: Reliability and validity. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 162–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koss, M. P., Gidycz, C. A., & Wisniewski, N. (1987). The scope of rape: Incidence and prevalence of sexual aggression and victimization in a national sample of higher education students. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 162–170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lefkowitz, E. S., Gillen, M. M., Shearer, C. L., & Boone, T. L. (2004). Religiosity, sexual behaviors, and sexual attitudes during emerging adulthood. The Journal of Sex Research, 41, 150–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leigh, B. C., Aramburu, B., & Norris, J. (1992). The morning after: Gender differences in attributions about alcohol-related sexual encounters. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, 343–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lipton, D. N., McDonel, E. C., McFall, R. M. (1987). Heterosocial perception of rapists. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 17–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malamuth, N. M. (1986). Predictors of naturalistic sexual aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(5), 953–962.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Metzler, C. W., Noell, J., & Biglan, A. (1992). The valuation of a construct of high-risk sexual behavior in heterosexual adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Research, 7, 233–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norris, J., Nurius, P. S., & Graham, T. L. (1999). When a date changes from fun to dangerous: Factors affecting a women's ability to distinguish. Violence and Victims, 5, 230–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seal, D. W., & Agostinelli, G. (1996). College students’ perceptions of the prevalence of risky sexual behavior. AIDS Care, 8, 453–466.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson, J. A., & Gangestad, S. W. (1991). Individual differences in sociosexuality: Evidence for convergent and discriminant validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 870–883.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Soler-Baillo, J. M., Marx, B. P., & Sloan, D. M. (2005). The psychophysiological correlates of risk recognition among victims and non-victims of sexual assault. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 169– 181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, E. U., Blais, A., & Betz, N. E. (2002). A domain-specific risk-attitudes scale: Measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15, 263–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, N. D. (1987). Unrealistic optimism about illness susceptibility: Conclusions from a community-wide sample. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 10(5), 481–500.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, N. D., & Klein, W. M. (1995). Resistance of personal risk perceptions to debiasing interventions. Health Psychology, 14, 132–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, A. E., Calhoun, K. S., & Bernat, J. A. (1999). Risk recognition and trauma-related symptoms among sexually revictimized women. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 705–710.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yeater, E. A., McFall, R. M., & Viken, R. J. (2004). The relationship between decision skills and women's risk for sexual victimization. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Zuckerman, M. (1991). Sensation seeking: The balance between risk and reward. In L. P. Lipsitt & L. L. Mitnick (Eds.), Self-regulatory behavior and risk taking: Causes and consequences (pp. 143–152). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I would like to thank two of my colleagues Drs. William R. Miller and Michael J. Dougher for reading the manuscript and providing helpful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elizabeth A. Yeater.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Yeater, E.A., Viken, R.J., McFall, R.M. et al. Sexual Attitudes and Instructional Set Affect Estimates of Risk and Response Effectiveness. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 28, 232–241 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-005-9018-1

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-005-9018-1

Keywords

Navigation