Journal of Logic, Language and Information

, Volume 24, Issue 4, pp 409–455 | Cite as

How Diagrams Can Support Syllogistic Reasoning: An Experimental Study

Article

Abstract

This paper explores the question of what makes diagrammatic representations effective for human logical reasoning, focusing on how Euler diagrams support syllogistic reasoning. It is widely held that diagrammatic representations aid intuitive understanding of logical reasoning. In the psychological literature, however, it is still controversial whether and how Euler diagrams can aid untrained people to successfully conduct logical reasoning such as set-theoretic and syllogistic reasoning. To challenge the negative view, we build on the findings of modern diagrammatic logic and introduce an Euler-style diagrammatic representation system that is designed to avoid problems inherent to a traditional version of Euler diagrams. It is hypothesized that Euler diagrams are effective not only in interpreting sentential premises but also in reasoning about semantic structures implicit in given sentences. To test the hypothesis, we compared Euler diagrams with other types of diagrams having different syntactic or semantic properties. Experiment compared the difference in performance between syllogistic reasoning with Euler diagrams and Venn diagrams. Additional analysis examined the case of a linear variant of Euler diagrams, in which set-relationships are represented by one-dimensional lines. The experimental results provide evidence supporting our hypothesis. It is argued that the efficacy of diagrams in supporting syllogistic reasoning crucially depends on the way they represent the relational information contained in categorical sentences.

Keywords

Efficacy of diagrams Human reasoning Logic and cognition Human experimentation Quantification Categorical syllogism External representation 

References

  1. Alter, A. L., Oppenheimer, D. M., Epley, N., & Eyre, R. N. (2007). Overcoming intuition: Metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 569–576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baron, M. E. (1969). A note on the historical development of logic diagrams: Leibniz, Euler and Venn. The Mathematical Gazette, 53, 113–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barwise, J., & Cooper, R. (1981). Generalized quantifiers and natural language. Linguistics and Philosophy, 4, 159–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barwise, J., & Etchemendy, J. (1991/1996). Visual information and valid reasoning. In W. Zimmerman & S. Cunningham (Eds.), Visualization in teaching and learning mathematics, MAA Notes, Number 19 (pp. 9–24). Reprinted in G. Allwein & J. Barwise (Eds). (1996). Logical reasoning with diagrams (pp. 3–26). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Barwise, J., & Etchemendy, J. (1994). Hyperproof, CSLI Lecture Notes, No. 42. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
  6. Barwise, J., & Hammer, E. (1996). Diagrams and the concept of logical system. In G. Allwein & J. Barwise (Eds.), Logical reasoning with diagrams (pp. 49–78). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Bauer, M., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1993). How diagrams can improve reasoning. Psychological Science, 4, 372–378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Benoy, F., & Rodgers, P. (2007). Evaluating the comprehension of Euler diagrams. In E. Banissi, et al. (Eds.), Proceedings of information visualization 2007 (pp. 771–778). Los Alamitos, CA: IEEE Computer Society.Google Scholar
  9. Blackburn, P., & Bos, J. (2005). Representation and inference for natural language: A first course in computational semantics. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
  10. Boolos, G. (1984). On ‘syllogistic inference’. Cognition, 17, 181–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bucciarelli, M., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1999). Strategies in syllogistic reasoning. Cognitive Science, 23, 247–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Calvillo, D. P., DeLeeuw, K., & Revlin, R. (2006). Deduction with Euler circles: Diagrams that hurt. In D. Baker-Plummer, R. Cox, & N. Swoboda (Eds.), Proceedings of Diagrams 2006, LNAI 4045 (pp. 199–203). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer.Google Scholar
  13. Camp, E. (2007). Thinking with maps. Philosophical Perspectives, 21, 145–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Chapman, L., & Chapman, J. (1959). Atmosphere effect re-examined. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 220–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Chapman, P., Stapleton, G., Rodgers, P., Micallef, L., & Blake, A. (2014). Visualizing sets: An empirical comparison of diagram types. In T. Dwyer, H. Purchase, & A. Delaney (Eds.), Proceedings of Diagrams 2014, LNAI 8578 (pp. 146–160). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer.Google Scholar
  16. Cheng, P. C.-H. (2004). Why diagrams are (sometimes) six times easier than words: Benefits beyond locational indexing. In A. F. Blackwell, K. Marriott, & A. Shimojima (Eds.), Proceedings of Diagrams 2004, LNAI 2980 (pp. 242–260). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer.Google Scholar
  17. Deloache, J. S., Sugarman, S., & Brown, A. L. (1985). The development of error correction strategies in young children’s manipulative play. Child Development, 56, 928–939.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dickstein, L. S. (1978). The effect of figure on syllogistic reasoning. Memory and Cognition, 6, 76–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Dickstein, L. S. (1981). The meaning of conversion in syllogistic reasoning. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 18, 135–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Dobson, M. (1999). Information enforcement and learning with interactive graphical systems. Learning and Instruction, 9, 365–390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Doumas, L. A., Hummel, J. E., & Sandhofer, C. M. (2008). A theory of the discovery and predication of relational concepts. Psychological Review, 115, 1–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Edwards, A. W. F. (2004). Cogwheels of the mind: The story of venn diagrams. Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
  23. Englebretsen, G. (1992). Linear diagrams for syllogisms (with relationals). Notre Dame Journal of Formal Logic, 33, 37–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Erickson, J. R. (1974). A set analysis theory of behavior in formal syllogistic reasoning tasks. In R. Solso (Ed.), Loyola symposium on cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 305–329). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  25. Euler, L. (1768). Lettres à une Princesse d’Allemagne sur Divers Sujets de Physique et de Philosophie. Saint-Pétersbourg: De l’Académie des Sciences.Google Scholar
  26. Evans, J St B T. (2003). In two minds: Dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 454–459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Evans, J. S. B. T. (2008). Dual processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255–278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Evans, J. S. B. T., Newstead, S. E., & Byrne, R. J. (1993). Human reasoning: The psychology of deduction. Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  29. Fish, A., Khazaei, B., & Roast, C. (2011). User-comprehension of Euler diagrams. Journal of Visual Languages and Computing, 22, 340–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Ford, M. (1994). Two modes of mental representation and problem solution in syllogistic reasoning. Cognition, 54, 1–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Gattis, M. (2005). Inferencing from spatial information. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 5, 119–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Gergonne, J. D. (1817). Essai de dialectique rationelle. Annuales de Mathematiques Pures et Appliqukes, 7, 189–228.Google Scholar
  33. Geis, M. L., & Zwicky, A. M. (1971). On invited inferences. Linguistic Inquiry, 2, 561–566.Google Scholar
  34. Geurts, B. (2003). Reasoning with quantifiers. Cognition, 86, 223–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Geurts, B. (2007). Existential import. In I. Comorovski & K. von Heusinger (Eds.), Existence: Semantics and syntax (pp. 253–271). Netherlands: Springer.Google Scholar
  36. Glasgow, J., Narayanan, N. H., & Chandrasekaran, B. (Eds.). (1995). Diagrammatic reasoning: Cognitive & computational perspectives. Cambridge, MA: AAAI Press/MIT Press.Google Scholar
  37. Greenfield, P. M., Nelson, K., & Saltzman, E. (1972). The development of rulebound strategies for manipulating seriated cups: A parallel between action and grammar. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 291–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Gurr, C. A., Lee, J., & Stenning, K. (1998). Theories of diagrammatic reasoning: Distinguishing component problems. Minds and Machines, 8, 533–557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., & Phillips, S. (1998). Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 803–831.Google Scholar
  40. Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., & Phillips, S. (2010). Relational knowledge: The foundation of higher cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 497–505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Hammer, E., & Shin, S. (1998). Euler’s visual logic. History and Philosophy of Logic, 19, 1–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Hegarty, M. (2004). Mechanical reasoning by mental simulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 280–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Heiser, J., & Tversky, B. (2006). Arrows in comprehending and producing mechanical diagrams. Cognitive Science, 30, 581–592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Hertzum, M., & Frøkjær, E. (1996). Browsing and querying in online documentation: A study of user interfaces and the interaction process. ACM Transactions on Computer–Human Interaction, 3, 136–161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Hodes, H. T. (1984). Logicism and the ontological commitments of arithmetic. Journal of Philosophy, 81, 123–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Horn, L. R. (2000). From if to iff: Conditional perfection as pragmatic strengthening. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 289–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Howse, J., Molina, F., Shin, S.-J., & Taylor, J. (2002). On diagram tokens and types. In M. Hegarty, B. Meyer, & N. H. Narayanan (Eds.), Proceedings of Diagrams 2002, LNAI 2317 (pp. 146–160). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer.Google Scholar
  48. Howse, J., Stapleton, G., & Taylor, J. (2005). Spider diagrams. LMS Journal of Computation and Mathematics, 8, 145–194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and reason. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  50. Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Towards cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  51. Johnson-Laird, P. N., & Byrne, R. (1991). Deduction. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  52. Johnson-Pynn, J., Fragaszy, D. M., Hirsh, E. M., Brakke, K. E., & Greenfield, P. M. (1999). Strategies used to combine seriated cups by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), bonobos (Pan paniscus), and capuchins (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 137–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Jones, S., McInnes, S., & Staveley, M. S. (1999). A graphical user interface for Boolean query specification. International Journal of Digital Library, 2, 207–223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Kneale, W., & Kneale, M. (1962). The development of logic. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  55. Khemlani, S., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2012). Theories of the syllogism: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 427–457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Lambert, J. H. (1764). Neues Organon oder Gedanken über die Erforschung und Bezeichnung des Wahren und dessen Unterscheidung vom Irrtum und Schein. Leipzig: Johann Wendler.Google Scholar
  57. Lakoff, G., & Nún̈ez, R. E. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  58. Larkin, J., & Simon, H. (1987). Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth 10,000 words. Cognitive Science, 11, 65–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Leibniz, G.W. (1903/1988). Opuscules et fragments inèdits: extraits des manuscrits de la bibliothèque royale de Hanovre. L. Couturat (ed). Hildesheim: Georg Olms Verlag.Google Scholar
  60. Lemon, O., & Pratt, I. (1998). On the insufficiency of linear diagrams for syllogisms. Notre Dame Journal of Formal Logic, 39, 573–580.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Leslie, S. J., & Gelman, S. A. (2012). Quantified statements are recalled as generics: Evidence from preschool children and adults. Cognitive Psychology, 64, 186–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Lull, R. (1617). Opera Ea Quae Ad Adinventam Ab Ipso Artem Universalem, Scientiarum Artiumque Omnium Breui Compendio, Firmaq́; Memoria Apprehendendarum, Locupletiffimaq́; Vel Oratione Ex Tempore Pertractandarum, Pertinent. Strassburg: Lazari Zetzneri.Google Scholar
  63. MacFarlane, J. (2002). Frege, Kant, and the logic in logicism. The Philosophical Review, 111, 25–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Meilinger, T., & Knauff, M. (2008). Ask for directions or use a map: A field experiment on spatial orientation and wayfinding in an urban environment. Journal of Spatial Science, 53, 13–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Michard, A. (1982). Graphical presentation of boolean expressions in a database query language: Design notes and an ergonomic evaluation. Behaviour and Information Technology, 1, 279–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Mineshima, K., Okada, M., Sato, Y., & Takemura, R. (2008). Diagrammatic reasoning system with Euler circles: Theory and experiment design. In G. Stapleton, J. Howse, & J. Lee (Eds.), Proceedings of Diagrams 2008, LNAI 5223 (pp. 188–205). Berlin Heidelberg: Springer.Google Scholar
  67. Mineshima, K., Okada, M., & Takemura, R. (2012a). A diagrammatic reasoning system with Euler circles. Journal of Logic, Language and Information, 21, 365–391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Mineshima, K., Okada, M., & Takemura, R. (2012b). A generalized syllogistic inference system based on inclusion and exclusion relations. Studia Logica, 100, 753–785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Mineshima, K., Sato, Y., Takemura, R., & Okada, M. (2014). Towards explaining the cognitive efficacy of Euler diagrams in syllogistic reasoning: A relational perspective. Journal of Visual Languages and Computing, 25, 156–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Monaghan, P., & Stenning, K. (1998). Effects of representational modality and thinking style on learning to solve reasoning problems. In M. A. Gernsbacher & S. J. Derry (Eds.), Proceedings of the 20th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 716–721). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  71. Myers, K. L., & Konolige, K. (1994). Reasoning with analogical representations. In G. Lakemeyer & B. Nebel (Eds.), Foundations of knowledge representation and reasoning, LNAI 810 (pp. 229–249). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Newstead, S. E., & Griggs, R. (1983). Drawing inferences from quantified statements: A study of the square of opposition. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 535–546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Palmer, S. (1978). Fundamental aspects of cognitive representation. In E. Rosch & B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 259–303). New Jersey: Lawrence Elbaum.Google Scholar
  74. Peirce, C.S. (1897/1933). Collected papers of Charles Sanders Peirce IV. In C. Hartshorne & P. Weiss (Eds.), Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  75. Pfeifer, N., & Kleiter, G. D. (2005). Towards a mental probability logic. Psychologica Belgica, 45, 71–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Politzer, G., van der Henst, J.-B., Luche, C. D., & Noveck, I. A. (2006). The interpretation of classically quantified sentences: A set-theoretic approach. Cognitive Science, 30, 691–723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Politzer, G., & Mercier, H. (2008). Solving categorical syllogisms with singular premises. Thinking and Reasoning, 14, 414–453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Pratt, I. (1993). Map semantics. In A. U. Frank & I. Campari (Eds.), Spatial information theory: A theoretical basis for GIS, LNCS 716 (pp. 77–91). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Purchase, H. C. (2014). Twelve years of diagrams research. Journal of Visual Languages and Computing, 25, 57–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Rescorla, M. (2009). Predication and cartographic representation. Synthese, 169, 175–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Revlis, R. (1975). Two models of syllogistic reasoning: Feature selection and conversion. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 180–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Rizzo, A., & Palmonari, M. (2005). The mediating role of artifacts in deductive reasoning. In B. G. Bara, L. Barsalou & M. Bucciarelli (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 1862–1867). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  83. Roberts, M. J., Newstead, S. E., & Griggs, R. A. (2001). Quanntifier interpretation and syllogistic reasoning. Thinking and Reasoning, 7, 173–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Rodgers, P. (2014). A survey of Euler diagrams. Journal of Visual Languages and Computing, 25, 134–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Sato, Y., Mineshima, K., & Takemura, R. (2010). Constructing internal diagrammatic proofs from external logic diagrams. In R. Catrambone & S. Ohlsson (Eds.), Proceedings of the 32nd annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 2668–2673). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  86. Sato, Y., Mineshima, K., & Takemura, R. (2011). Interpreting logic diagrams: A comparison of two formulations of diagrammatic representations. In C. Hoelscher, T. F. Shipley & L. Carlson (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 2182–2187). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  87. Sato, Y., Masuda, S., Someya, Y., Tsujii, T., & Watanabe, S. (2015). An fMRI analysis of the efficacy of Euler diagrams in logical reasoning. In Proceedings of 2015 IEEE symposium on visual languages and human-centric computing (9 pp). Los Alamitos, CA: IEEE Computer Society Press.Google Scholar
  88. Scaife, M., & Rogers, Y. (1996). External cognition: How do graphical representations work? International Journal of Human–Computer Studies, 45, 185–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Shimojima, A. (1996). On the efficacy of representation. PhD thesis, Indiana University.Google Scholar
  90. Shimojima, A., & Katagiri, Y. (2013). An eye-tracking study of exploitations of spatial constraints in diagrammatic reasoning. Cognitive Science, 37, 211–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Shin, S.-J. (1994). The logical status of diagrams. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  92. Stapleton, G. (2005). A survey of reasoning systems based on Euler diagrams. Proceedings of Euler diagrams 2004, ENTCS 134 (pp. 127–151). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  93. Stenning, K. (2002). Seeing reason: Image and language in learning to think. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Stenning, K., Cox, R., & Oberlander, J. (1995). Contrasting the cognitive effects of graphical and sentential logic teaching: Reasoning, representation and individual differences. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10, 333–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Stenning, K., & Oberlander, J. (1995). A cognitive theory of graphical and linguistic reasoning. Cognitive Science, 19, 97–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Stenning, K., & Lemon, O. (2001). Aligning logical and psychological perspectives on diagrammatic reasoning. Artificial Intelligence Review, 15, 29–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Stenning, K., & van Lambalgen, M. (2001). Semantics as a foundation for psychology: A case study of Wason’s selection task. Journal of Logic, Language and Information, 10, 273–317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Stenning, K., & van Lambalgen, M. (2004). A little logic goes a long way: Basing experiment on semantic theory in the cognitive science of conditional reasoning. Cognitive Science, 28, 481–529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Stenning, K., & van Lambalgen, M. (2008). Human reasoning and cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  100. Sugimoto, Y., & Sato, Y. (2015). A specification-aware modeling of syllogistic reasoning with mental models. In N. A. Taatgen, M. K. van Vugt, J. P. Borst & K. Mehlhorn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 13th international conference on cognitive modeling (pp. 31–36). Groningen, The Netherlands: University of Groningen.Google Scholar
  101. Szymanik, J., & Zajenkowski, M. (2010). Comprehension of simple quantifiers: Empirical evaluation of a computational model. Cognitive Science, 34, 521–532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Takemura, R. (2013). Proof theory for reasoning with Euler diagrams: A logic translation and normalization. Studia Logica, 101, 157–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Venn, J. (1881). Symbolic logic. London: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Westerståhl, D. (1989). Quantifiers in formal and natural languages. In D. Gabbay & F. Guenthner (Eds.), Handbook of philosophical logic (Vol. 4, pp. 2–132). Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
  105. Zhang, J., & Norman, D. A. (1994). Representations in distributed cognitive tasks. Cognitive Science, 18, 87–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of General Systems Studies, Graduate School of Arts and SciencesThe University of TokyoTokyoJapan
  2. 2.Center for Simulation SciencesOchanomizu UniversityTokyoJapan

Personalised recommendations