Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The Intergenerational Transmission of Attitudes: Analyzing Time Preferences and Reciprocity

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Family and Economic Issues Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present research adds to the question on intergenerational correlation of attitudes between parents and children. So far, it is not clear whether the transmission process is purely genetic or whether parents take an active role in socializing their children. The transmission of time preferences and reciprocity is analyzed by focusing on three aspects: (1) direct transmission from parents to children; (2) the impact of prevailling attitudes in children’s surrounding environment; (3) parents’ positive assortative mating. The findings support all three channels. Differences in the size of the intergenerational correlation according to family or parental characteristics suggest that the process is not purely genetic. The present analysis is of even greater importance, as the analyzed attitudes determine economic success sustainably.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Paternalism captures the extent to which parents disagree with the natural preferences of their children and try to interfere with their own choices (Zilibotti and Doepke 2014).

  2. Version 29, SOEP, 2013, doi:10.5684/soep.v29.

  3. There was no information on all these attitudes for all of the individuals, as some individuals who where interviewed in 2008 did not answer some of the questions in 2010 and vice versa. Summing up, there were 2395 observations for patience and 2394 for impulsiveness. Furthermore there were 2246 observations for positive and 2221 observations for negative reciprocity, for children and both of their parents.

  4. German and English versions of the questionnaires are available at www.diw.de.

  5. The six items were based on the measure developed by Perugini et al. (2003) which consists of 27 items, measuring reciprocity as well as beliefs on reciprocity. A shortened version with the six items mentioned above was used in the main SOEP survey.

  6. Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient was used as the attitudes were measured on an ordinal scale (Conover 1995, p. 245).

  7. No alternative estimation techniques were used as Dohmen et al. (2012) showed in their analysis of intergenerational correlation that results from OLS estimations were robust to using other estimation techniques such as IV regression or using a binary measure as the dependent variable.

  8. For all subsequent analysis, further results on the regressions including only father’s or mother’s attitude are available upon request.

  9. Those variables have been found to determine not only patience, but also positive and negative reciprocity (see e.g., Dohmen et al. 2008; Vischer et al. 2013) and were therefore included in all following analysis.

  10. On a scale ranking from 0—“risk averse” to 10—“fully prepared to take risks” individuals had to assess whether they are fully prepared to take risks or try to avoid them. Validity for the use of the general risk question was given by Dohmen et al. (2011) who showed that responses to this question were good predictions of the actual risk behavior of individuals in a lottery experiment.

  11. There were several missing observation for years of schooling in the data set. To avoid dropping these observations, all missings were replaced with the mean of the schooling-variable and an indicator for missing schooling information was included. Additionally, (unreported) specifications included indicator variables for school-leaving degree (upper secondary or technical school degree, intermediate school degree, secondary school degree, dropout, missing) instead of years of schooling. The results were qualitatively and quantitatively similar.

  12. The SOEP included information on the households’ region of residence on the level of the spatial planning regions (in German: Raumordnungsregionen, ROR). Germany was divided into 97 spatial planning regions, which were defined by the Federal Office for Building and Regional Planning. Thus, it was possible to add variables that indicated in which of these regions survey participants lived at the time of the interview in the analysis (for more detailed information see Knies and Spiess 2007).

  13. The measure of trust came from three questions from the 2008 wave of the SOEP which were combined into a single item measuring generalized trust (Richter et al. 2013). In the original three questions individuals had to state their agreement on general trust, reliance on others, and need for caution in dealing with strangers using a four-point scale (1-“Agree completely”—4 -“Disagree completely”).

  14. For an overview on measurement errors see Greene (2012).

  15. Detailed results available upon request.

  16. The aggregation of the attitudes on the regional level has to be treated carefully, as one has to keep in mind that the SOEP data should not be taken as representative of the whole regional population (Knies and Spiess 2007).

  17. The results for those regressions are available upon request.

  18. The SOEP included two questions on whether one had arguments or fights with the mother or father at age 15. The five answering categories range from “very often” to “never”. If children indicated that they fought “sometimes”, “seldom”, or “never” with one of their parents during their childhood, the relationship was seen as harmonic. If children indicated that they fought “often” or “very often”, the relationship was called “Fights” in Part 9-4.

  19. Overall life satisfaction was measured with a single item, where individuals had to indicate on a scale from 0 to 10 whether they are completely dissatisfied or completely satisfied. When mother’s or father’s life satisfaction was above the average satisfaction of all parents in the sample, it was seen as “High”, otherwise as “Low” in Table 9.

  20. According to Stock and Watson (2007) the F-test for a single endogenous regressor should be above 10, otherwise the instruments are assumed to be weak.

References

  • Andreoni, J., & Sprenger, C. (2012). Risk preferences are not time preferences. American Economic Review, 201(7), 3357–3376. doi:10.1257/aer.102.7.3357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anger, S. (2012). The intergenerational transmission of cognitive and non-cognitive skills during adolescence and young adulthood. SOEPpapers on Multidisciplinary Panel Data Research, 473. doi:10.2139/ssrn.2142491.

  • Anger, S., & Heineck, G. (2010). Do smart parents raise smart children? The intergenerational transmission of cognitive abilities. Journal of Population Economics, 23(3), 1105–1132. doi:10.1007/s00148-009-0298-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, G. S. (1976). The economic approach to human behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, G. S. (1993). A treatise on the family. Cambridge: Harvard university press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & Mc-Cabe, K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity and social history. Games and Economic Behavior, 10, 122–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bisin, A., & Verdier, T. (2000). “Beyond the Melting Pot”: Cultural transmission, marriage, and the evolution of ethnic and religious traits. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 115, 955–988. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/2586900

  • Blanden, J., Gregg, P., & Macmillan, L. (2007). Accounting for intergenerational income persistence: Noncognitive skills, ability and education. The Economic Journal, 117, 43–60. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0297.2007.02034.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (2002). The inheritance of inequality. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 16(3), 3–30. doi:10.1257/089533002760278686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1988). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Castillo, M., Ferraro, P. J., Jordan, J. L., & Petrie, R. (2011). The today and tomorrow of kids: Time preferences and educational outcomes of children. Journal of Public Economics, 95, 1377–1385. doi:10.1016/j.jpubeco.2011.07.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chuang, Y., & Schechter, L. (2015). Stability of experimental and survey measures of risk, time, and social preferences: A review and some new results. Journal of Development Economics, 117, 151–170. doi:10.1016/j.jdeveco.2015.07.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conover, W. J. (1995). Practical nonparametric statistics. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., & Sunde, U. (2008). Representative trust and reciprocity: Prevalence and determinants. Economic Inquiry, 46, 84–90. doi:10.1111/j.1465-7295.2007.00082.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., & Sunde, U. (2009). Homo reciprocans: Survey evidence on behavior outcomes. The Economic Journal, 119, 592–612. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0297.2008.02242.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., & Sunde, U. (2012). The intergenerational transmission of risk and trust attitudes. Review of Economic Studies, 79, 645–677. doi:10.1093/restud/rdr027.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., Sunde, U., Schupp, J., & Wagner, G. G. (2011). Individual risk attitudes: Measurement, determinants and behavioral consequences. Journal of the European Economic Association, 9, 522–550. doi:10.1111/j.1542-4774.2011.01015.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, G., Kalil, A., Mayer, S. E., Tepper, R., & Payne, M. R. (2005). The apple does not fall far from the tree. In S. Bowles, H. Gintis, & M. Osborne Groves (Eds.), Unequal chances: Family background and economic success (pp. 23–79). Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eaves, L., Heath, A., Martin, N., Maes, H., Neale, M., Kendler, K., et al. (1999). Comparing the biological and cultural inheritance of personality and social attitudes in the Virginia 30 000 study of twins and their relatives. Twin Research, 2(02), 62–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E. & Gächter, S. (2000). Fairness and retaliation: The economics of reciprocity. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 141, 159-181. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/2646924

  • Figueredo, A. J., Defcek, J. A., & Jones, D. N. (2006). The ideal romantic partner personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 431–441. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.02.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finke, M. S., & Huston, S. J. (2013). Time preference and the importance of saving for retirement. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 89, 23–34. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2013.03.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ge, X., Conger, R. D., Cadoret, R. J., Neiderhiser, J. M., Yates, W., Troughton, E., et al. (1996). The developmental interface between nature and nurture: a mutual influence model of child antisocial behavior and parent behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 32(4), 574. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.32.4.574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, W. H. (2012). Econometric analysis. New York: Pearson Education Limited.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guttman, J. M. (2000). On the evolutionary stability of preferences for reciprocity. European Journal of Political Economy, 16(1), 31–50. doi:10.1016/S0176-2680(99)00046-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haisken-DeNew, J. P., & Frick, J. R. (2005). Desktop companion to the German Socio-Economic Panel Study (SOEP) - version 8.0. update to Wave 21. German Institute for Economic Research.

  • Heckmann, J. J., Stixrud, J., & Urzua, S. (2006). The effects of cognitive and noncognitive abilities on labor market outcomes and social behavior. Journal of Labor Economics, 24(3), 411–482. doi:10.3386/w12006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hertz, T., Jayasundera, T., Piraino, P., Selcuk, S., Smith, N., & Verashchagina, A. (2008). The inheritance of educational inequality: International comparisons and fifty-year trends. The B.E. Journal of Economic Analysis & Policy, 7. doi:10.2202/1935-1682.1775.

  • Kirby, K. N., Winston, G. C., & Santiesteban, M. (2005). Impatience and grades: Delay-discount rates correlate negatively with college GPA. Learning and Individual Differences, 15, 213–222. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2005.01.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knies, G., & Spiess, C. K. (2007). Regional Data in the German Socio-Economic Panel Study (SOEP). DIW Berlin - Data Documentation, 17.

  • Lawless, L. J. R., Nayga, R., & Drichoutis, A. (2013). Time preference and health behaviour: A review. Agricultural and Food Economics, 1, 1–17. doi:10.1186/2193-7532-1-17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meier, S., & Sprenger, C. (2010). Present-biased preferences and credit card borrowing. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 193–210. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/25760198

  • Meier, S., & Sprenger, C. (2015). Temporal stability of time preferences. Review of Economics and Statistics, 97(2), 273–286. doi:10.1162/REST_a_00433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perugini, M., Gallucci, M., Presaghi, F., & Ercolani, A. P. (2003). The personal norm of reciprocity. European Journal of Personality, 17, 251–283. doi:10.1002/per.474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richter, D., Metzing, M., Weinhardt, M., & Schupp, J. (2013). SOEP scales manual. SOEP survey papers, 138.

  • Solon, G. (2002). Cross-country differences in intergenerational earnings mobility. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 16(3), 59–66. doi:10.1257/089533002760278712.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stock, J. H., & Watson, M. W. (2007). Introduction to econometrics. New York: Pearson Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tidd, K. L., & Lochard, J. S. (1978). Monetary significance of the affiliative smile: A case for reciprocal altruism. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 11, 344–346. doi:10.3758/BF03336849.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vischer, T., Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffmann, D., Schupp, J., Sunde, U., et al. (2013). Validating an ultra-short survey measure of patience. Economic Letters, 120, 142–145. doi:10.1016/j.econlet.2013.04.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, G., Burkhauser, R. V., & Behringer, F. (1993). The english language public use file of the German socio-economic panel study. The Journals of Human Resources, 28(2), 429–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zilibotti, F., & Doepke, M. (2014). Parenting with style: Altruism and paternalism in intergenerational preference transmission. 2014 Meeting Papers, No. 343, Society for Economic Dynamic.

  • Zumbuehl, M., Dohmen, T., & Pfann, G. A. (2013). Parental investment and the intergenerational transmission of economic preferences and attitudes. IZA Discussion Papers, 7476. doi:10.2139/ssrn.2304090.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Britta Gauly.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gauly, B. The Intergenerational Transmission of Attitudes: Analyzing Time Preferences and Reciprocity. J Fam Econ Iss 38, 293–312 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10834-016-9513-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10834-016-9513-4

Keywords

Navigation