Skip to main content
Log in

Latino Families Engage in Elaborative Conversations: Effects on Children’s Recall and Vocabulary

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Child and Family Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Objective

This study examined whether training Latina mothers, during a home visiting program, to engage in elaborative reminiscing (ER) with their preschool-aged children would lead to improvements in their children’s recall of events and vocabulary.

Methods

Three-year-old children and their mothers were randomly assigned to an ER group or an active control group. During a 4-month period, mothers in the ER group used strategies to elicit personal narratives from their child. Over the same period, mothers in a control group engaged in games and activities with their child.

Results

At the 4-month posttest, mothers who participated in ER asked fewer closed-ended questions about a past event than controls (Cohen d = 0.49). Their children’s personal narratives included more descriptive and nonverbal enactment event details than the control children (ds ranged from 0.53 to 0.56). A year later, children in the ER group had significantly higher receptive English vocabulary scores than controls (d = 0.83). The possibility for transfer of mothers’ ER strategies to book reading was also examined, but no significant effects were found.

Conclusions

Results support the feasibility and benefits of an elaborative conversations intervention for Latino parents and children. Gains in children’s narrative skills were consistent with prior research in which Latino families construct stories in culturally specific ways.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anthony, J. L., Solari, E. J., Williams, J. M., Schoger, K. D., Zhang, Z., Branum-Martin, L., & Francis, D. J. (2009). Development of bilingual phonological awareness in Spanish-speaking English language learners: the roles of vocabulary, letter knowledge, and prior phonological awareness. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 535–564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atwill, K., Blanchard, J., Christie, J., Gorin, J. S., & Garcia, H. S. (2010). English-language learners: implications of limited vocabulary for cross-language transfer of phonemic awareness with kindergartners. Journal of Hispanic Higher Education, 9, 104–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, R. H. (1993). Children’s home environments, health, behavior, and intervention efforts: a review using the HOME inventory as a marker measure. Genetic, Social and General Psychology Monographs, 119, 437–490.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, R. H., & Caldwell, B. M. (1979). Home observation for measurement of the environment: a revision of the preschool scale. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 84, 235–244.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, R. H., Caldwell, B. M., & Rock, S. L. (1988). Home environment and school performance: a ten-year follow-up and examination of three models of environmental action. Child Development, 59, 852–867.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Caldwell, B. M., & Bradley, R. H. (2003). Home observation for measurement of the environment: administration manual. Tempe, AZ: Family & Human Dynamics Research Institute, Arizona State University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carmiol, A., & Sparks, A. (2014). Narrative development across cultural contexts: Finding the pragmatic in parent-child reminiscing. In D. Mathews (Ed.), Pragmatic development: trends in language acquisition research. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carmiol, A. M., Sparks, A., & Conejo, L. D. (2017). Reminiscing and book sharing and their links with children’s early literacy development in Costa Rica. Paper presented at the Many Paths to Language Workshop at the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.

  • Carvajal, H. H., Parks, J. P., Looan, R. A., & Page, G. L. (1992). Comparisons of the IQ and vocabulary scores on Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence – Revised and Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – Revised. Psychology in the Schools, 29, 22–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caspe, M.(2009). Low-income Latino mothers’ booksharing styles and children’s emergent literacy development. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 24, 306–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chaudry, A., & Wimer, C. (2016). Poverty is not just an indicator: The relationship between income, poverty, and child well-being. Academic Pediatrics, 16, 23–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis. 2nd edn. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, D. K. & Tabors, P. O. (Eds.) (2001). Beginning literacy with language: young children’s learning at home and school. Baltimore, MD, US: Paul H Brookes Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (2007). The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Fourth edn. Bloomington, MN: NCS Pearson, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, A. R. (1985). Learning to describe past experiences in conversation. Discourse Processes, 8, 177–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farrant, K., & Reese, E. (2000). Maternal style and children’s participation in reminiscing: stepping stones in children’s autobiographical memory development. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1, 193–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fivush, R., & Fromhoff, F. A. (1988). Style and structure in mother–child conversations about the past. Discourse Processes, 11, 337–355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fivush, R., Haden, C. A., & Reese, E. (2006). Elaborating on elaborations: role of maternal reminiscing style in cognitive and socioemotional development. Child Development, 77, 1568–1588.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Francis, D., Carlson, C., Fletcher, J., Foorman, B., Goldenberg, C., Vaughn, S., & Papanicolaou, A. (2005). Oracy/literacy development of Spanish-speaking children: a multi-level program of research on language minority children and the instruction, school and community contexts, and interventions that influence their academic outcomes. Perspectives, 31, 8–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, E. E., & Garcia, E. H. (2012). Understanding the language development and early education of hispanic children. New York: Teachers College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haden, C. A., & Fivush, R. (1996). Contextual variation in maternal conversational styles. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42, 24–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haden, C. A., Haine, R. A., & Fivush, R. (1997). Developing narrative structure in parent-child reminiscing across the preschool years. Developmental Psychology, 33, 295–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hodapp, A. F. (1993). Correlation between Stanford-Binet IV and PPVT-R scores for young children. Psychological Reports, 73, 1152–1154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, J. A. (1993). Reminiscing with mothers and others: Autobiographical memory in young two-year-olds. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 3, 1–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16, 367–371.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lamb, M. E., Hershkowitz, I., Sternberg, K. J., Boat, B., & Everson, M. D. (1996). Investigative interviews of alleged sexual abuse victims with and without anatomical dolls. Child Abuse and Neglect, 20, 1251–1259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., & Guttentag, C. (2008). A responsive parenting intervention: the optimal timing across early childhood for impacting maternal behaviors and child outcomes. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1335–1353.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Laible, D. (2004). Mother-child discourse surrounding a child’s past behavior at 30 months: links to emotional understanding and early conscience development at 36 months. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 50, 159–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leyva, D., & Nolivos, V. (2015). Chilean family reminiscing about emotions and its relation to children’s self-regulation skills. Early Education and Development, 26, 770–791.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leyva, D., Sparks, A., & Reese, E. (2012). The link between preschooler’s phonological awareness and mother’s book reading and reminiscing practices in low-income families. Journal of Literacy Research, 44, 426–447. https://doi.org/10.1177/1086296X12460040.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCabe, A., & Peterson, C. (1991). Getting the story: a longitudinal study of parental styles in eliciting narratives and developing narrative skill. In A. McCabe & C. Peterson (Eds.), Developing narrative structure. (pp. 217–253). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  • McWayne, C. M., Melzi, G., Limlingan, M. C., & Schick, A. (2016). Ecocultural patterns of family engagement mong low-income Latino families of preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 52, 1088–1102.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Gopnik, A. (1989). On linking nonverbal imitation, representation, and language learning in the first two years of life. In G. E. Speidel & K. E. Nelson (Eds.), The many faces of imitation in language learning (pp. 23–51). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Melzi, G. (2000). Cultural variations in the construction of personal narratives: Central American and European American mothers’ elicitation styles. Discourse Processes, 30, 153–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melzi, G., Schick, A. R., & Kennedy, J. L. (2011). Narrative elaboration and participation: two dimensions of maternal elicitation style. Child Development, 82, 1282–1296.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. M., Potts, R., Fung, H., Hoogstra, L., & Mintz, J. (1990). Narrative practices and the social construction of self in childhood. American Ethnologist, 17, 292–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neale, D., & Pino-Pasternak, D. (2016). A review of reminiscing in early childhood settings and links to sustained shared thinking. Educational Psychology Review, 29, 641–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neha, T., Reese, E., Schaughency, E., & Taumoepeau, M. (2018). The role of whānau Māori (New Zealand Māori families) for Māori children’s early learning. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Niemeyer, A. E., Wong, M. M., & Westerhaus, K. J. (2009). Parental involvement, familismo, and academic performance in Hispanic and Caucasian adolescents. North American Journal of Psychology, 11, 613–631.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (2004). Echoing our parents: Parental influences on children's narration. In M. W. Pratt & B. H. Fiese (Eds.), Family stories and the life course: Across time and generations (pp. 27–54). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

  • Peterson, C., Jesso, B., & McCabe, A. (1999). Encouraging narratives in preschoolers: an intervention study. Journal of Child Language, 26, 49–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1994). A social interactionist account of developing contextualized narrative skill. Developmental Psychology, 30, 937–948.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raikes, H., Pan, B. A., Luze, G., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Brooks-Gunn, J., Constantine, J., Banks Tarullo, L., Raikes, H. A., & Rodriguez, E. T. (2006). Mother-child book reading in low-income families: Correlates and outcomes during the first three years of life. Child Development, 77, 924–953.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reese, E. (1995). Predicting children’s literacy from mother–child conversations. Cognitive Development, 10, 381–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reese, E., Haden, C. A., & Fivush, R. (1993). Mother–child conversations about the past: relationships of style and memory over time. Cognitive Development, 8, 403–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reese, E., Leyva, D., Sparks, A., & Grolnick, W. (2010). Maternal elaborative reminiscing increases low income children’s narrative skills relative to dialogic reading. Early Education and Development, 21, 318–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reese, E., & Newcombe, R. (2007). Training mothers in elaborative reminiscing enhances children’s autobiographical memory and narrative. Child Development, 78, 1153–1170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, M. L. (2013). Decontexualized language input and preschoolers’ vocabulary development. Seminars in Speech and Language, 34, 260–266.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, M. L., & Goldwin-Meadow, S. (2009). Early gesture selectively predicts later language learning. Developmental Science, 12, 182–187. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00764.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2017). Does far training? Negative evidence from chess, music, and working memory training. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26, 515–520.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, M. R., & Dadds, M. R. (1982). The effects of planned activities and child management training: an analysis of setting generality. Behavior Therapy, 13, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, M. R., & Glynn, E. L. (1981). Training parents in behavioral self-management: an analysis of generalization and maintenance effects. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 14, 223–237.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Silva, M., & McCabe, A. (1996) Vignettes of the continuous and family ties: Some Latino American traditions. In A. McCabe (Ed.), Chameleon readers: Teaching children to appreciate all kinds of good stories. (pp. 116–136). New York: McGraw-Hill.

  • Skolnick-Weisberg, D., Hande, I., Hirsch-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R., Nicolopoulu, A., & Dickinson, D. K. (2015). Shovels and swords: how realistic and fantastical themes affect children’s word learning. Cognitive Development, 35, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, A., & Reese, E. (2013). From reminiscing to reading: Home contributions to children’s developing language and literacy in low-income families. First Language, 33, 89–110. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723711433583.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thierry, K. L., Lamb, M. E., Orbach, Y., & Pipe, M.-E. (2006). Developmental differences in the function and use of anatomical dolls during interviews with alleged sexual abuse victims. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 1125–1134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thierry, K. L., & Spence, M. J. (2004). A real-life event enhances the accuracy of preschoolers’ recall. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 297–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thierry, K. L., Spence, M. J., & Memon, A. (2001). Before misinformation is encountered: source monitoring decreases child witness suggestibility. Journal of Cognition and Development, 2, 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompkins, V., Bengochea, A., Nicol, S., & Justice, L. M. (2017). Maternal inferential input and children’s language skills. Reading Research Quarterly, 52, 397–416. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Totsika, V., & Sylva, K. (2004). The home observation for measurement of the environment revisited. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 9, 25–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • U.S. Bureau of the Census (2018). Current Population Survey Table Creator, www.census.gov.

  • Waddell, M. (1992). Owl babies. Somerville: Candlewick Press.

  • Wareham, P., & Salmon, K. (2006). Mother-child reminiscing about everyday experiences: implications for psychological interventions in the preschool years. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 535–554.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K.T. (2007). The expressive vocabulary test, 2nd edn. Bloomington, MN: NCS Pearson, Inc.

  • Willems, M. (2004). Knufflebunny: A cautionary tale. New York: Hyperion Books.

  • Yarosz, D. J., & Barnett, W. S. (2001). Who reads to young children? identifying predictors of family reading activities. Reading Psychology, 22, 67–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank the children and parents who participated in this research. We also thank Monica Arellano for her support with intervention training and Veronica Angyal for her assistance with data coding.

Author Contributions

K.T. designed the study, managed execution of the study, completed data analyses, and helped to write the paper. A.S. collaborated on the design of the study and writing of the paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Karen L. Thierry.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Informed Consent

Informed consent and assent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Research Involving Human Participants

Institutional Review Board (IRB) approval was obtained from the IRB committee of Momentous Institute. All study procedures were conducted in accordance with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Additional information

Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Thierry, K.L., Sparks, A. Latino Families Engage in Elaborative Conversations: Effects on Children’s Recall and Vocabulary. J Child Fam Stud 28, 2297–2311 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-019-01446-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-019-01446-0

Keywords

Navigation