Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Mentalization in Chilean Mothers with Children Aged 12 and 30 Months: Relation to Child Sex and Temperament and Family Socioeconomic Status

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Child and Family Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Objectives

Maternal mentalization refers to a mother’s ability to consider her child as a subject with a mind and reflect this understanding through references to mental states in her speech with the child. Mentalization is relevant for a child’s attachment and theory of mind. The aim of this research is to describe mentalization among a group of Chilean mothers with their children at two time periods −12 and 30 months of age-, analyze developmental changes between both ages and examine the relationship among maternal mentalization, the child’s sex and temperament and the family socioeconomic status.

Methods

The research used a sample of 90 dyads, and the instruments utilized include a sociodemographic questionnaire, a mentalization assessment and Rothbart’s temperament questionnaires (IBQ-R-VSF and ECBQ-VSF).

Results

The results showed a decline in references to desires and physical states and an increase in causal talk and cognitions at 30 months of age compared to 12 months. In 12-month-old children, only the temperament dimension of negative affect demonstrated a relationship with factual talk and SES related to emotions. Whereas at 30 months, the dimensions of extraversion, regulation and child’s sex related to references to desires, causal talk, emotions and physical states. The mothers’ socioeconomic status (SES) related to references to desires.

Conclusions

The best predictor of change in mentalization between both ages was maternal education level.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adimark. (2000). El nivel socioeconómico ESOMAR. Manual de aplicación [Socioeconomic level ESOMAR, application Manual]. Santiago, Chile: Adimark GfK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong, L. M. (2011). Emotion language in early childhood: relations with children’s emotion regulation strategy understanding and emotional self-regulation (Doctoral dissertation, The Pennsylvania State University).

  • Brophy-Herb, H., Stansbury, K., Bocknek, E., & Horodynski, M. (2012). Modeling maternal emotion-related socialization behaviors in a low-income sample: Relations with toddlers’ self-regulation. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(3), 352–364. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.11.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. R., & Dunn, J. (1996). Continuities in emotion understanding from three to six years. Child Development, 67(3), 789–802. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131861.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chile Crece Contigo (2015). Programa de apoyo al desarrollo biopsicosocial (PADB) y Programa de apoyo al recién nacido(a) (PARN). Catálogo de prestaciones 2015. [Support program to biopsychosocial development (PABD) and support program, to newborn (PARN). 2015 benefits catalogue].

  • Chaplin, T. M., & Aldao, A. (2013). Gender differences in emotion expression in children: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 735–765. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030737.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cova, S., Valdivia, P., & Maganto, M. (2005). Diferencias de género en psicopatología en la niñez: Hipótesis explicativas [Gender differences in infancy’s psychopathology: Explicative hypotheses]. Revista chilena Délelőtt pediatría, 76(4), 418–424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, D. K., Hofer, K. G., Barnes, E. M., & Grifenhagen, J. F. (2014). Examining teachers’ language in head Start classrooms from a systemic linguistics approach. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(3), 231–244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.02.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ensink, K., Normandin, L., Plamondon, A., Berthelot, N., & Fonagy, P. (2016). Intergenerational pathways from reflective functioning to infant attachment through parenting. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science/Revue Canadienne Des Sciences Délután Comportement, 48(1), 9–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/cbs0000030.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farkas, C., Carvacho, C., Galleguillos, F., Montoya, F., León, F., Santelices, M. P., & Himmel, E. (2015). Estudio comparativo de la sensibilidad entre madres y personal educativo en interacción con niños y niñas de un año de edad [Comparative study between mothers’ and teachers’ sensitivity in interaction with one-year-old children]. Perfiles Educativos, 37(148), 16–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farkas, C., del Real, M. T., Strasser, K., Álvarez, C., Santelices, M. P., & Sieverson, C. (2018). Maternal mental state language during storytelling versus free-play contexts and its relation to child language and socioemotional outcomes at 12 and 30 months of age. Cognitive Development, 47, 181–197. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.06.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farkas, C., Strasser, K., Badilla, G. & Santelices, M. P. (2017). Mentalization in Chilean educational staff with 12-month-old children: Does it make a difference in relation to what children receive at home? Early Education and Development. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2017.1287994.

  • Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E., & Target, M. (2002). Affect regulation, mentalization, and the development of the self. New York: Other Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frampton, K., Perlman, M., & Jenkins, J. (2009). Caregivers’ use of metacognitive language in child care centers: Prevalence and predictors. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 24(3), 248–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Studying infant temperament via the revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development, 26(1), 64–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00169-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jessee, A., McElwain, N. L., & Booth-LaForce, C. (2016). Maternal supportive behavior, cognitive talk, and desire/emotion talk at 24 months: Distinct factors and differential antecedents. Parenting, 16(2), 63–86. https://doi.org/10.1080/15295192.2016.1134988.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jonn-Seed, M. S., & Weiss, S. J. (2005). Maternal state of mind and expressed emotion: Impact of mothers’ mental health, stress, and family satisfaction. Journal of the American Psychiatric Nurses Association, 11(3), 135–143. https://doi.org/10.1177/1078390305279025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kivijärvi, M., Räihä, H., Kaljonen, A., Tamminen, T., & Piha, J. (2005). Infant temperament and maternal sensitivity behavior in the first year of life. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46(1), 421–428.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LePage, H. D. (2003). Effects of single parents’ gender role attitudes on adolescents’ gender role attitudes (Order No. 3097847). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (305270625). http://ezproxy.puc.cl/docview/305270625?accountid=16788.

  • Manlove, E. E., & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2002). Caring for infant daughters and sons in dual-earner households: Maternal reports of father involvement in weekday time and tasks. Infant and Child Development, 11(4), 305–320. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manrique, D., Ghesquière, P., & Van Leeuwen, K. (2014). Parenting, socioeconomic status and psychosocial functioning in Peruvian families and their children. Anales Délelőtt Psicología, 30(3), 995–1005.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martínez-Fuentes, M. T., Brito de la Nuez, A. G., & Pérez-López, J. (2000). Temperamento del niño y personalidad de la madre como antecedentes de la seguridad del apego [Child’s temperament and mother’s personality as antecedents of attachment security]. Anuario Délelőtt psicología/The Ubung Journal of Psychology, 31(1), 25–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meins, E., Centifanti, L. C. M., Fernyhough, C., & Fishburn, S. (2013). Maternal mind-mindedness and children’s behavioral difficulties: Mitigating the impact of low socioeconomic status. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(4), 543–553. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9699-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Arnott, B., Leekam, S. R., & Rosnay, M. (2013). Mind‐mindedness and theory of mind: mediating roles of language and perspectival symbolic play. Child Development, 84(5), 1777–1790.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., De Rosnay, M., Arnott, B., Leekam, S. R., & Turner, M. (2012). Mind‐mindedness as a multidimensional construct: Appropriate and non-attuned mind‐related comments independently predict infant–mother attachment in a socially diverse sample. Infancy, 17(4), 393–415.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Russel, J., & Clark-Carter, D. (1998). Security of attachment as a predictor of symbolic and mentalising abilities: A longitudinal study. Social Development, 7, 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00047.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murnen, S. K., & Don, B. P. (2012). Body image and gender roles. Encyclopedia of Body Image and Human Appearance, 1, 128–134. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-384925-0.00019-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olhaberry, M. (2012). Interacciones tempranas y género infantil en familias monoparentales chilenas. Revista Latinoamericana Délelőtt Psicología, 44(2), 75–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papalia, D. E., & Feldman, R. D. (2012). Desarrollo humano [Human development]. 12° edition Santiago, Chile: McGraw Hill Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascal, B., Aguardo, G., Sotillo, M., & Masdeu, J. C. (2008). Acquisition of mental state language in Spanish children: A longitudinal study of the relationship between the production of mental verbs and linguistic development. Developmental Science, 11(4), 454–466. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00691.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, S. P., Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler temperament: the early childhood behavior questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development, 29(3), 386–401. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.01.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, S. P., Helbig, A. L., Gartstein, M. A., Rothbart, M. K., & Leerkes, E. (2014). Development and assessment of short and very short forms of the infant behavior questionnaire-revised. Journal of Personality Assessment, 96(4), 445–458. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2013.841171.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, S. P., Jacobs, J., Gartstein, M. A. & Rothbart, M. K. (2010). Development and assessment of short and very short forms of the Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire. Poster presented at International Conference on Infant Studies, Baltimore.

  • Rothbart, M. K. (2004). Commentary: differentiated measures of temperament and multiple pathways to childhood disorders. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33(1), 82–87. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, P. (1981). Development of individual differences in temperament. In M. E. Lamb & A. Brown (Eds.), Advances in Developmental Psychology (pp. 37–86). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, M. (2008). Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language, 35, 185–205. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000907008343.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, M., & Casillas, A. (2011). Parental goals and talk with toddlers. Infant and Child Development, 20(5), 475–494. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.709.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Santelices, M. P., Farkas, C., Montoya, M. F., Galleguillos, F., Carvacho, C., Fernández, A., Morales, L., Taboada, C., & Himmel, E. (2015). Factores predictivos de sensibilidad materna en infancia temprana [Predictive factors of maternal sensitivity in early infancy]. Psicoperspectivas, 14(1), 66–76. https://doi.org/10.5027/PSICOPERSPECTIVAS-VOL14-ISSUE1-FULLTEXT-441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwab, J. F., & Lew-Williams, C. (2016). Language learning, socioeconomic status, and child-directed speech. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews, 7(4), 264–275. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1393.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schoppe‐Sullivan, S. J., Diener, M. L., Mangelsdorf, S. C., Brown, G. L., McHale, J. L., & Frosch, C. A. (2006). Attachment and sensitivity in family context: the roles of parent and infant gender. Infant and Child Development, 15(4), 367–385. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sharp, C., & Fonagy, G. (2008). The parent’s capacity to treat the child as a psychological agent: Constructs, measures and implications for developmental psychopathology. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slade, A., Grienenberger, J., Bernbach, E., Levy, D., & Locker, A. (2005). Maternal reflective functioning, attachment, and the transmission gap: a preliminary study. Attachment and Human Development, 7(3), 283–298. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730500245880.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stich, M., Girolametto, L., Johnson, C. J., Cleave, P. L., & Chen, X. (2015). Contextual effects on the conversations of mothers and their children with language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(2), 323–344. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716413000258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Susperreguy, M. I., Strasser, K., Lissi, M. R., & Mendive, S. (2007). Creencias y prácticas de literacidad en familias chilenas con distintos niveles educativos [Beliefs and practices of literacy in Chilean families with different levels of education]. Revista Latinoamericana Délelőtt Psicología, 39(2), 239–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taumoepeau, M., & Ruffman, T. (2006). Mother and infant talk about mental states relates to desire language and emotion understanding. Child Development, 77(2), 465–481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taumoepeau, M., & Ruffman, T. (2008). Stepping stones to others’ minds: Maternal talk relates to child mental state language and emotion understanding at 15, 24, and 33 months. Child Development, 79(2), 284–302. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01126.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, R. B., & Foster, B. J. (2014). Socioeconomic status and parent-child relationships predict metacognitive questions to preschoolers. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 43(4), 315–33. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-013-9256-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Kleeck, A., Vander Woude, J., & Hammett, L. (2006). Fostering literal and inferential language skills in head start preschoolers with language impairment using scripted book-sharing discussions. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 15(1), 85–95. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2006/009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wei, H. S., & Chen, J. K. (2013). The relationships between family financial stress, mental health problems, child rearing practice, and school involvement among Taiwanese parents with school-aged children. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 23(7), 1145–1154. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-013-9772-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woldarsky, V., Urzúa, C., Farkas, C. & Vallotton, C. D. (2018). Differences in maternal sensitivity responses according to child gender and temperament at 12 months old: a comparative study between Chile and USA. Manuscript under review.

Download references

Author Contributions

C.A. guided the edition process of the manuscript and collaborated with the literature research. P.C. and M.T.D.R. assisted with the literature research. C.F. executed the analyses and helped with the design of the paper. The writing of the paper was a collaboration of the team.

Funding

This research was supported by grant from the Fondo Nacional de Desarrollo Científico y Tecnológico (FONDECYT N° 1110087 and N°1160110).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Carolina Álvarez.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile research committee and The American Psychological Association and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the authors.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Álvarez, C., Cristi, P., Del Real, M.T. et al. Mentalization in Chilean Mothers with Children Aged 12 and 30 Months: Relation to Child Sex and Temperament and Family Socioeconomic Status. J Child Fam Stud 28, 959–970 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-019-01348-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-019-01348-1

Keywords

Navigation