Skip to main content
Log in

Family Exploration: The Contribution of Stability and Change Processes

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Child and Family Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Building upon the redefinition of exploration as a family process, this study analyses how the processes of family stability and change may favour exploration by members of multiple family relational contexts. Sixty non-clinical family triads (mother, father, child) participated in an experimental observational study and were video-recorded while playing in different interactive configurations. The children (37 females and 23 males) were 4–5 years old (M = 55 months). The mothers’ ages ranged from 29 to 45 (M = 38) and the fathers’ ages ranged from 29 to 46 (M = 39). All the parents were employed and were living together. All participants were Caucasians of Italian nationality. Using the Triadic Interactions Analytical Procedure (TIAP), the family morphostatic processes and the family morphogenetic processes were analysed in relation to the family members’ exploration. Data analyses showed that family stability continual construction (morphostasis) and family change (morphogenesis) involve different interactive and relational dynamics (χ²(8) = 13.84, p > .05; CFI = .97, TLI = .94, SRMR = .06), even if they are intertwined processes (p < .001). Both morphostatic and morphogenetic processes were correlated to the level of exploration showed by family members (respectively r = .32, p < .05, and r = .59, p < .001), even if the morphogenetic processes had a stronger relation with family exploration (z = 1.85, p one-tailed  < .05).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ainsworth, M. (1972). Attachment and dependency: A comparison. In J. Gewirtz (Ed.), Attachment and dependency (pp. 97–137). Washington, D.C.: Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Bell, S. M. (1970). Attachment, exploration, and separation: Illustrated by the behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation. Child Development, 41, 49–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baiocco, R., Cacioppo, M., Laghi, F., & Tafà, M. (2013). Factorial and construct validity of FACES IV among Italian adolescents. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 22, 962–970.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Belsky, J., Rovine, M., & Fish, M. (1989). The developing family system. In M. R. Gunnar & E. Thelen (Eds.), Systems and development. The Minnesota symposia of child development (pp. 119–166). Hilsday NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human development. New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burns, M. E., & Pearson, J. C. (2011). An exploration of family communication environment, everyday talk and family satisfaction. Communication Studies, 62, 171–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byng-Hall, J. (1995a). Creating a secure family base: Some implication of attachment theory for family therapy. Family Process, 34, 45–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Byng-Hall, J. (1995b). Rewriting family scripts. London: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caffery, T., & Erdam, P. (2003). Attachment and family systems theories: Implications for family therapists. Journal of Systemic Therapies, 22(2), 3–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cigala, A., Fruggeri, L., & Venturelli, E. (2013). Family microtransitions: An observational study. Marriage & Family Review, 49(8), 717–736.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cigala, A., Fruggeri, L., & Venturelli, E. (2015). Family configurations and microtransitions: Promoting change e reconstructing stability in triadic interactions. Psicologia Clinica dello Sviluppo, 1, 125–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cigala, A., Venturelli, E., & Fruggeri, L. (2014). Family functioning in microtransition and socio-emotional competence in preschoolers. Early Child Development and Care, 184(4), 553–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conger, R. D., Lorenz, F. O. & Wickrama, K. A. S. (Eds.) (2004). Continuity and change in family relations: Theory, methods and empirical findings. Mahwah. NJ: Laurence Erlabaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Wolff, M. S., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development, 68(4), 571–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donley, M. G. (1993). Attachment and the emotional unit. Family Process, 32, 3–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, N. B., Baldwin, L. M., & Bishop, D. S. (1983). The Mc Master family assessment device. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 9(2), 171–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, N., Bishop, D., & Baldwin, L. M. (1982). McMaster model of family functioning. A view of the normal family. In F. Walsh (Ed.), Normal family process (1st ed., pp. 115–191). New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Everri, M., Mancini, T., & Fruggeri, L. (2016). The role of rigidity in adaptive and maladaptive families assessed by FACES IV: The points of view of adolescents. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25(10), 2987–2997.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Falicov, C. J. (1988). Family transitions: Continuity and change over the life cycle. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiese, B. H. (2002). Routines of daily living and rituals in family life: A glimpse of stability and change during the early child-raising years. Zero to Three, 22(4), 10–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiese, B. H. (2006). Family routines and rituals. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fivaz-Depeursinge, E., & Corboz-Warney, A. (1999). The primary triangle: A developmental systems view of mothers, fathers, and infants. New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franklin, C., Streeter, C. L., & Springer, D. W. (2001). Validity of the FACES IV family assessment measure. Research on Social Work Practice, 11, 576–596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, W. A., & Easterbrooks, M. A. (1984). The role of marital quality in toddler development. Developmental Psychology, 20, 504–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, I., & Feldman, R. (2008). Synchrony in the Triad: A microlevel process model of coparenting and parent-child interactions. Family Process, 47, 465–479.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hilburn-Cobb, C. (1996). Adolescent-parent attachments family problem-solving styles. Family Process, 35(1), 57–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, J., Fonagy, P., Safier, E., & Sarfent, J. (2003). The ecology of attachment in the family. Family Process, 42, 205–221.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, L. (1971). Deviation-amplifying processes in natural groups. In J. Haley (Ed), Changing families (pp. 285–311). New York, NY: Grune and Strutton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kreppner, K. (2009). Analyzing deep structures of family processes. In L. Fruggeri (Ed.), Observing families: Methods and techniques (pp. 41–70). Roma (Italy): Carocci.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lebow, J., & Stroud, C. B. (2012). Assessment of effective couple and family functioning: Prevailing model and instruments. In F. Walsh (Ed.), Normal family process growing diversity and complexity (4th ed., pp. 501–528). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, F. O., Wickrama, K. A. S., & Conger, R. D. (2004). Modelling continuity and change in family relation in panel data. In R.D. Conger, F. O. Lorenz & K. A. S. Wickrama (Eds.), Continuity and change in family relations: Theory, methods and empirical findings (pp. 15–63). Mahwah, NJ: Laurence Erlabaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGoldrick, M., Carter, B., & Garcia Preto, N. (2011). Overview: The life cycle in its changing context. In M. McGoldrick, B. Carter & N. Garcia Preto (Eds.), The expanded family life cycle: Individual, family, and social perspective (4th ed., pp. 1–19). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Margolin, G., Oliver, P. H., Gordis, E. B., Garcia O’Hearn, H., Medina, A. M., Ghosh, C. M., & Morland, L. (1998). The nuts and bolts of behavioral observations of marital and family interaction. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 1(4), 195–213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maruyama, M. (1963). The second cybernetics: Deviation-amplifying mutual causal processes. American Scientist., 51(2), 164–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marvin, B. S., & Stewart, B. B. (1990). A family system framework for the study of attachment. In M. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti & M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment beyond the preschool years (pp. 51–86). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. (2006). Mplus user’s guide (version 4). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minuchin, S. (1974). Families and family therapy. Cambridge, Mass., Harvard: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory (2nd ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norén, K., & Linell, P. (2007). Meanings potentials and the interaction between lexis and contexts: An empirical substantiation. Pragmatics, 17, 387–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D. H. (2000). Circumplex model of family systems. Journal of Family Therapy, 22(2), 144–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D. H. (2008). FACES IV Manual. Minneapolis (MN): Life Innovations.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiss, D. (1981). The family’s construction of reality. Cambridge, Mass., Harvard: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (2007). A primer in conversation analysis. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Speer, M. T. (1970). Family systems: morphostasis and morphogenesis. Family Process., 9, 259–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson-Hinde, J. (1990). Attachment within family systems: An overview. Infant Mental Health Journal, 11, 218–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stupica, B., Sherman, L. J., & Cassidy, J. (2011). Newborn irritability moderates the association between infant attachment security and toddler exploration and sociability. Child Development, 82(5), 1381–1389.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Venturelli, E., Cabrini, E., Fruggeri, L., & Cigala, A. (2016). The study of triadic family interactions: The proposal of an observational procedure. Integrative Psychological Behavioral Science, 50, 655–683. doi:10.1007/s12124-015-9335-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, F. (2016). A family developmental framework: challenges and resilience across the family life cycle. In T. L. Sexton & J. Lebow (Eds.), Handbook of Family Therapy (pp. 30–47). New York and London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watzlavick, P., Weakland, J., & Fish, R. (1974). Change. New York, NY: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author Contributions

A.C.: designed and executed the study, analyzed the data and wrote the paper. T.M.: designed and executed the study, analyzed the data and wrote the paper. E.V.: designed and executed the study, analyzed the data and wrote the paper. L.F.: designed and executed the study, analyzed the data and wrote the paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ada Cigala.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all adult participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Cigala, A., Mancini, T., Venturelli, E. et al. Family Exploration: The Contribution of Stability and Change Processes. J Child Fam Stud 27, 154–165 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-017-0860-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-017-0860-z

Keywords

Navigation