Adrián, J. E., Clemente, R. A., Villanueva, L., & Rieffe, C. (2005). Parent-child picture-book reading, mothers’ mental state language and children’s theory of mind. Journal of Child Language,
32, 673–686.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Arranz, E., Artamedi, J., Olabarrieta, F., & Martín, J. (2002). Family context and theory of mind development. Early Child Development and Care,
172, 9–22.
Article
Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition,
21, 37–46.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Bates, J. E., & McFadyen-Ketchum, S. (2001). Temperament and parent-child relations as interacting factors in children’s behavioral adjustment. In V. J. Molfese & D. L. Molfese (Eds.), Temperament and personality development across the life span (pp. 141–176). Mahwah: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental Psychology Monographs,
4, 1–103.
Article
Google Scholar
Bennett, D. S., Bendersky, M., & Lewis, M. (2005). Antecedents of emotion knowledge: Predictors of individual differences in young children. Cognition and Emotion,
19, 375–396.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Blissett, J. (2011). Relationships between parenting style, feeding style and feeding practices and fruit and vegetable consumption in early childhood. Appetite,
57, 826–831.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Arterberry, M. E., Mash, C., & Manian, N. (2011). Discrimination of facial expression by 5-month-old infants of nondepressed and clinically depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and Development,
34, 100–106.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Brown, L., & Iyengar, S. (2008). Parenting styles: The impact on student achievement. Marriage & Family Review,
43, 14–38.
Article
Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development,
72, 1032–1053.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Carpendale, J., & Lewis, C. (2006). How children develop social understanding. Malden: Blackwell Publishing.
Google Scholar
Carrick, N., Quas, J. A., & Lyon, T. D. (2010). Maltreated and nonmaltreated children’s evaluations of emotional fantasy. Child Abuse and Neglect,
34, 129–134.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Cassano, M., Perry-Parrish, C., & Zeman, J. (2007). Influence of gender on parental socialization of children’s sadness regulation. Social Development,
16, 2010–2231.
Article
Google Scholar
Cassidy, J., Parke, R. D., Butkovsky, L., & Braungart, J. M. (1992). Family-peer connections: The roles of emotional expressiveness within the family and children’s understanding of emotions. Child Development,
63, 603–618.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Chaplin, T. M., Casey, J., Sinha, R., & Mayes, L. C. (2010). Gender differences in caregiver emotion socialization of low-income toddlers. In A. Kennedy Root & S. Denham (Eds.), The role of gender in the socialization of emotion: Key concepts and critical issues. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, (Vol. 128, pp. 11–27). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development,
15, 481–498.
Article
Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., Maughan, A., Toth, S. L., & Bruce, J. (2003). False belief understanding in maltreated children. Development and Psychopathology,
15, 1067–1091.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Cole, K., & Mitchell, P. (1998). Family background in relation to deceptive ability and understanding of the mind. Social Development,
7, 181–197.
Article
Google Scholar
de Rosnay, M., & Harris, P. (2002). Individual differences in children’s understanding of emotion: The roles of attachment and language. Attachment & Human Development,
4, 39–54.
Article
Google Scholar
de Rosnay, M., & Hughes, C. (2006). Conversation and theory of mind: Do children talk their way to socio-cognitive understanding? British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
24, 7–37.
Article
Google Scholar
Denham, S. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior, and emotion in preschoolers: Contextual validation. Child Development,
57, 194–201.
Article
Google Scholar
Denham, S. A., Bassett, H. H., & Wyatt, T. M. (2010). Gender differences in the socialization of preschoolers’ emotional competence. In A. Kennedy Root & S. Denham (Eds.), The role of gender in the socialization of emotion: Key concepts and critical issues. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development (Vol. 128, pp. 29–49). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Denham, S., McKinley, M., Couchoud, E., & Holt, R. (1990). Emotional and behavioral predictors of preschool peer ratings. Child Development,
61, 1145–1152.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Denham, S. A., Mitchell-Copeland, J., Strandberg, K., Auerbach, S., & Blair, K. (1997). Parental contributions to preschoolers emotional competence: Direct and indirect effects. Motivation and Emotion,
21, 65–86.
Article
Google Scholar
Denham, S. A., Zoller, D., & Couchoud, E. A. (1994). Socialization of preschoolers’ emotion understanding. Developmental Psychology,
30, 928–936.
Article
Google Scholar
Dunn, J., Brown, J., & Beardsall, L. (1991a). Family talk about feeling states and children’s later understanding of others’ emotions. Child Development,
27, 448–455.
Google Scholar
Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L. (1991b). Young children’s understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development,
62, 1352–1366.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Edwards, A., Shipman, K., & Brown, A. (2005). The socialization of emotional understanding: A comparison of neglectful and nonneglectful mothers and their children. Child Maltreatment,
10, 293–304.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Ensor, R., & Hughes, C. (2008). Content or connectedness? Mother-child talk and early social understanding. Child Development,
79, 201–216.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Ensor, R., Spencer, D., & Hughes, C. (2011). ‘You feel sad?’ Emotion understanding mediates effects of verbal ability and mother-child mutuality on prosocial behaviors: Findings from 2 years to 4 years. Social Development,
20, 93–110.
Article
Google Scholar
Ereky-Stevens, K. (2008). Associations between mothers’ sensitivity to their infants’ internal states and children’s later understanding of mind and emotion. Infant and Child Development,
17, 527–543.
Article
Google Scholar
Fabes, R., Poulin, R., Eisenberg, N., & Madden-Derdich, D. (2002). The coping with children’s negative emotions scale (CCNES): Psychometric properties and relations with children’s emotional competence. Marriage and Family Review,
34, 285–310.
Article
Google Scholar
Farrant, B. M., Devine, T. A. J., Maybery, M. T., & Fletcher, J. (2011). Empathy, perspective taking and prosocial behaviour: The importance of parenting practices. Infant and Child Development,
21, 175–188.
Article
Google Scholar
Fonagy, P., Redfern, S., & Charman, T. (1997). The relationship between belief-desire reasoning and a projective measure of attachment security (SAT). British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
15, 51–61.
Article
Google Scholar
Garner, P. W., Jones, D. C., Gaddy, G., & Rennie, K. M. (1997). Low-income mothers’ conversations about emotions and their children’s emotional competence. Social Development,
9, 29–48.
Google Scholar
Garner, P. W., Jones, D. C., & Miner, J. L. (1994). Social competence among low-income preschoolers: Emotion socialization practices and social cognitive correlates. Child Development,
65, 622–637.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Garner, P. W., & Power, T. G. (1996). Preschoolers’ emotional control in the disappointment paradigm and its relation to temperament, emotional knowledge, and family expressiveness. Child Development,
67, 1406–1419.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Gotlib, I. H., & Goodman, S. H. (1999). Children of parents with depression. In W. K. Silverman & T. H. Ollendick (Eds.), Developmental issues in the clinical treatment of children and adolescents (pp. 415–432). New York: Allyn & Bacon.
Google Scholar
Greig, A., & Howe, D. (2001). Social understanding, attachment security of preschool children and maternal mental health. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
19, 381–393.
Article
Google Scholar
Guajardo, N. R., Snyder, G., & Petersen, R. (2009). Relationships among parenting practices, parental stress, child behaviour, and children’s social-cognitive development. Infant and Child Development,
18, 37–60.
Article
Google Scholar
Halberstadt, A. G., Crisp, V. W., & Eaton, K. L. (1999). Family expressiveness: A retrospective and new directions for research. In P. Philippot, R. S. Feldman, & E. J. Coats (Eds.), The social context of nonverbal behavior (pp. 109–155). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Happé, F. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development,
66, 843–855.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Harris, P. L., Donnelly, K., Guz, G. R., & Pitt-Watson, R. (1986). Children’s understanding of the distinction between real and apparent emotion. Child Development,
57, 895–909.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Hughes, C. (2011). Social understanding and social lives: From toddlerhood through to the transition to school. New York: Psychology Press.
Google Scholar
Hughes, C., Deater-Deckard, K., & Cutting, A. L. (1999). ‘Speak roughly to your little boy?’ Sex differences in the relations between parenting and preschoolers’ understanding of mind. Social Development,
8, 143–160.
Article
Google Scholar
Hughes, C., Dunn, J., & White, A. (1998). Trick or treat? Uneven understanding of mind and emotion and executive dysfunction in “hard-to-manage” preschoolers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines,
39, 981–994.
Article
Google Scholar
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2009). Independence and interplay between maternal and child risk factors for preschool problem behaviors. International Journal of Behavioral Development,
33, 1–11.
Article
Google Scholar
Hughes, C., & Plomin, R. (2000). Individual differences in early understanding of mind: Genes, nonshared environment and modularity. In P. Carruthers & A. Chamberlain (Eds.), Evolution and the human mind: Language, modularity and social cognition (pp. 47–61). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Johnson, W., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2007). How parents influence school grades: Hints from a sample of adoptive and biological families. Learning and Individual Differences,
17, 201–219.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Kawabata, Y., Alink, L. R. A., Tseng, W. L., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Crick, N. R. (2011). Maternal and paternal parenting styles associated with relational aggression in children and adolescents: A conceptual analysis and meta-analytic review. Developmental Review,
31, 240–278.
Article
Google Scholar
Kristen, S., Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., & Perst, H. (2011). Infants’ joint attention skills predict toddlers’ emerging mental state language. Developmental Psychology,
47, 1207–1219.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
LaBounty, J., Wellman, H. M., Olson, S., Lagattuta, L., & Liu, D. (2008). Mothers’ and fathers’ use of internal state talk with their young children. Social Development,
17, 757–775.
Article
Google Scholar
Laible, D. (2004a). Mother-child discourse surrounding a child’s past behavior at 30 months: Links to emotional understanding and early conscience development at 36 months. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,
50, 159–180.
Article
Google Scholar
Laible, D. (2004b). Mother-child discourse in two contexts: Links with child temperament, attachment security, and socioemotional competence. Developmental Psychology,
40, 979–992.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Laible, D. J., & Thompson, R. A. (1998). Attachment and emotional understanding in preschool children. Developmental Psychology,
34, 1038–1045.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Laranjo, J., Bernier, A., Meins, E., & Carlson, S. (2010). Early manifestations of children’s theory of mind: The role of mind-mindedness and infant security of attachment. Infancy,
15, 300–323.
Article
Google Scholar
Legerstee, M. (2005). Infants’ sense of people: Precursors to a theory of mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Masten, C. L., Guyer, A. E., Hodgdon, H. B., McClure, E. B., Charney, D. S., Ernst, M., et al. (2008). Recognition of facial emotions among maltreated children with high rates of post-traumatic stress disorder. Child Abuse and Neglect,
32, 139–153.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
McElwain, N. L., & Volling, B. L. (2004). Attachment security and parental sensitivity during infancy: Associations with friendship quality and false-belief understanding at age 4. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships,
21, 639–667.
Article
Google Scholar
Mcquaid, N., Bigelow, A. E., McLaughlin, J., & MacLean, K. (2008). Maternal mental state language and preschool children’s attachment security: Relation to children’s mental state language and expressions of emotional understanding. Social Development,
17, 61–83.
Article
Google Scholar
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Russell, J., & Clark-Carter, D. (1998). Security of attachment as a predictor of symbolic and metalising abilities: A longitudinal study. Social Development,
7, 1–24.
Article
Google Scholar
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Das Gupta, M., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Development,
73, 1715–1726.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Misailidi, P. (2006). Young children’s display rule knowledge: Understanding the distinction between apparent and real emotions and the motives underlying the use of display rules. Social Behavior and Personality,
34, 1285–1296.
Article
Google Scholar
Nelson, J. A., O’Brien, M., Calkins, S. D., Leerkes, E. M., Marcovitch, S., & Blankson, A. N. (2011) Maternal expressive style and children’s emotional development. Infant and Child Development. Advance online publication. doi:10.1002/icd.748.
Olson, S. L., Lopez-Duran, N., Lunkenheimer, E. S., Chang, H., & Sameroff, A. J. (2011). Individual differences in the development of early peer aggression: Integrating contributions of self-regulation, theory of mind and parenting. Development and Psychopathology,
23, 253–266.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Olson, J., & Masur, E. F. (2011). Infants’ gestures influence mothers’ provision of object action and internal state labels. Journal of Child Language,
38, 1028–1054.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Ontai, L. L., & Thompson, R. A. (2008). Attachment, parent-child discourse and theory-of-mind development. Social Development,
17(1), 47–60.
Article
Google Scholar
Pears, K. C., & Moses, L. J. (2003). Demographics, parenting and theory of mind in preschool children. Social Development,
12, 1–20.
Article
Google Scholar
Perlman, S., Camras, L. A., & Pelphrey, K. (2008a). A. Physiology and functioning: Parents’ vagal tone, emotion socialization, and children’s emotion knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
100, 308–315.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Perlman, S. B., Kalish, C. W., & Pollak, S. D. (2008b). The role of maltreatment experience in children’s understanding of the antecedents of emotion. Cognition and Emotion,
22, 651–670.
Article
Google Scholar
Peterson, C., & Slaughter, V. (2003). Opening windows into the mind: Mothers’ preferences for mental state explanations and children’s theory of mind. Cognitive Development,
18, 399–429.
Article
Google Scholar
Pinderhughes, E. E., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., & Zelli, A. (2000). Determinants of discipline decisions: Family background characteristics, stress, social support and parent cognitive/emotional processes. Journal of Family Psychology,
14, 380–400.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Pollak, S. D., Cicchetti, D., Hornung, K., & Reed, A. (2000). Recognizing emotion in faces: Developmental effects of child abuse and neglect. Developmental Psychology,
36, 679–688.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Pollak, S. D., Cicchetti, D., Klorman, R., & Brumaghim, J. (1997). Cognitive brain event-related potentials and emotion processing in maltreated children. Child Development,
68, 773–787.
Article
Google Scholar
Pollak, S. D., Klorman, R., Thatcher, J. E., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). P3b reflects maltreated children’s reactions to facial displays of emotion. Psychophysiology,
38, 267–274.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Pollak, S. D., & Sinha, P. (2002). Effects of early experience on children’s recognition of facial displays of emotion. Developmental Psychology,
38, 784–791.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Pollak, S. D., & Tolley-Schell, S. A. (2003). Selective attention to facial emotion in physically abused children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
112, 323–338.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Racine, T. P., Carpendale, J. I. M., & Turnbull, W. (2007). Parent-child talk and children’s understanding of beliefs and emotions. Cognition and Emotion,
21, 480–494.
Article
Google Scholar
Raikes, H. A., & Thompson, R. A. (2006). Family emotional climate, attachment security, and young children’s emotion knowledge in a high-risk sample. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
24, 89–104.
Article
Google Scholar
Repacholi, B., & Trapolini, T. (2004). Attachment and preschool children’s understanding of maternal versus non-maternal psychological states. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
22, 395–415.
Article
Google Scholar
Rieffe, C., Terwogt, M. M., Koops, W., Stegge, H., & Oomen, A. (2001). Preschoolers’ appreciation of uncommon desires and subsequent emotions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
19, 259–274.
Article
Google Scholar
Rohrer, L. M., Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., Toth, S. L., & Maughan, A. (2011). Effects of maternal negativity and of early and recent recurrent depressive disorder on children’s false belief understanding. Developmental Psychology,
47, 170–181.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Root, A. K., & Denham, S. (2010). Focus on gender: Parent and child contributions to the socialization of emotion [Special issue]. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development,
128, 1–109.
Article
Google Scholar
Ruffman, T., Perner, J., & Parkin, L. (1999). How parenting style affects false belief understanding. Social Development,
8, 395–441.
Article
Google Scholar
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children’s and mothers’ mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development,
73, 734–751.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., Devitt, K., & Crowe, E. (2006). What mothers say and what they do: The relationship between parenting, theory of mind, language and conflict/cooperation. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
24, 105–124.
Article
Google Scholar
Sabbagh, M. A., & Seamans, E. L. (2008). Intergenerational transmission of theory-of-mind. Developmental Science,
11, 354–360.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Salmon, C. A., & Shackelford, T. K. (Eds.). (2007). Family relationships: An evolutionary perspective. New York: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Shackman, J. E., Shackman, A. J., & Pollak, S. D. (2007). Physical abuse amplifies attention to threat and increases anxiety in children. Emotion,
7, 838–852.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Shipman, K., Edwards, A., Brown, A., Swisher, L., & Jennings, E. (2005). Managing emotion in a maltreating context: A pilot study examining child neglect. Child Abuse and Neglect,
29, 1015–1029.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Slaughter, V., Peterson, C. C., & Carpenter, M. (2008). Maternal talk about mental states and the emergence of joint visual attention. Infancy,
13, 640–659.
Article
Google Scholar
Slaughter, V., Peterson, C. C., & Mackintosh, E. (2007). Mind what mother says: Narrative input and theory of mind in typical children and those on the autism spectrum. Child Development,
78, 839–858.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Spera, C. (2005). A review of the relationship among parenting practices, parenting styles, and adolescent school achievement. Educational Psychology Review,
17, 125–146.
Article
Google Scholar
Steele, M., Steele, H., Croft, C., & Fonagy, P. (1999). Infant mother attachment at one year predicts children’s understanding of mixed emotions at 6 years. Social Development,
8, 161–178.
Article
Google Scholar
Stevenson, O. (2007). Neglected children and their families. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
Book
Google Scholar
Sullivan, M. W., Bennett, D. E., Carpenter, K., & Lewis, M. (2008). Emotional knowledge in young neglected children. Child Maltreatment,
13, 301–306.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Sullivan, M. W., Carmody, D. P., & Lewis, M. (2010). How neglect and punitiveness influence emotion knowledge. Child Psychiatry and Human Development,
41, 285–298.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Symons, D. K., & Clark, S. E. (2000). A longitudinal study of mother–child relationships and theory of mind in the preschool period. Social Development,
9, 3–23.
Article
Google Scholar
Symons, D., Fossum, K., & Collins, K. (2006). A longitudinal study of belief and desire state discourse during mother–child play and later false belief understanding. Social Development,
15, 676–691.
Article
Google Scholar
Taumoepeau, M., & Ruffman, T. (2006). Mother and infant talk about mental states relates to desire language and emotion understanding. Child Development,
77, 465–481.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Turnbull, W., Carpendale, J. I. M., & Racine, T. P. (2009). Talk and children’s understanding of mind. Journal of Consciousness Studies,
16, 140–166.
Google Scholar
Van Bergen, P., Salmon, K., Dadds, M. R., & Allen, J. (2009). The effect of mother training in emotion-rich, elaborative reminiscing on children’s shared recall and emotion knowledge. Journal of Cognition and Development,
10, 162–187.
Article
Google Scholar
Vinden, P. (2001). Parenting attitudes and children’s understanding of mind: A comparison of Korean-American and Anglo-American families. Cognitive Development,
16, 793–809.
Article
Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., Lane, J. D., LaBounty, J., & Olson, S. L. (2011). Observant, nonaggressive temperament predicts theory-of-mind development. Developmental Science,
14, 319–326.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition,
13, 103–128.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar