Skip to main content

Materiality and Human Cognition

Abstract

In this paper, we examine the role of materiality in human cognition. We address issues such as the ways in which brain functions may change in response to interactions with material forms, the attributes of material forms that may cause change in brain functions, and the spans of time required for brain functions to reorganize when interacting with material forms. We then contrast thinking through materiality with thinking about it. We discuss these in terms of their evolutionary significance and history as attested by stone tools and writing, material forms whose interaction endowed our lineage with conceptual thought and meta-awareness of conceptual domains.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Notes

  1. A similar analysis of the other writing systems thought to represent independent invention—those of Egypt, China, and Mesoamerica—has not been performed. Thus, they cannot presently be quantified in terms of the length of time needed to realize literacy. However, all four independent origins (the three mentioned plus Mesopotamia) were similar in being bureaucratic states, implying a similar production demand, i.e., repetition of conventionalized, simple, non-numerical signs by hand at a volume and over a duration of time sufficient to train the fusiform gyrus, garner handwriting effects, adapt the material form, and realize literacy; Overmann 2016a).

  2. Beyond common sense, there are few criteria for determining when an object functions as part of the cognitive system. For example, if reading does not exist as a cognitive activity without interacting with the material form of writing, it implies that a book is part of the cognitive system whenever someone is reading it. Its cognitive status while unused but recalled is less certain, its cognitive status unpurchased at a distant bookstore or unfinished by its author even more so. All these connections (and more) can conceivably mean that books are part of the cognitive system, and as concepts, they are certainly anchored by our experience of interacting with books.

  3. Embodied engagement with material objects involves neural activity (e.g., motor planning and execution), and motor planning in the absence of motor execution has been found in mentally manipulating concepts like numbers. It may be implicit to literacy as well, since Exner’s area for controlling the movements of handwriting is active in recognizing characters (i.e., reading), as distinct from their manual production (handwriting). We have proposed the term “neural muscle” for this phenomenon. Specific neural activity continues to be elicited by interactions with specific material forms, even as the original motor movements become obsolete and are discarded (e.g., as typing on computer keyboards obviates writing by hand). This suggests that neural activity associated with higher-level cognitive functions may relate to productive behavior with past cultural forms and behaviors (e.g., prehistoric stone tools and their production and use), with descendent interactions with cultural forms and behaviors perpetuating the associated neural responses. Elsewhere, we have proposed the term “neural fossil” for the persistence of “neural muscles” beyond the material forms that occasioned them (though “fossil” has an inapt connotation of formerly and hence no longer living). We propose that humans have developed a generalized neurological response to material culture that is perpetuated by interacting with descendent material forms and behaviors. None of this discussion should be understood as proposing that the neural activity in question is necessarily representational in nature.

References

  • Abutalebi, J. (2008). Neural aspects of second language representation and language control. Acta Psychologica, 128(3), 466–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andres, M., Seron, X., & Olivier, E. (2007). Contribution of hand motor circuits to counting. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(4), 563–576.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balsters, J. H., Whelan, C. D., Robertson, I. H., & Ramnani, N. (2013). Cerebellum and cognition: evidence for the encoding of higher order rules. Cerebral Cortex, 23(6), 1433–1443.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkai, R., Gopher, A., & LaPorta, P. (2006). Middle Pleistocene landscape of extraction: quarry and workshop complexes in northern Israel. In N. Goren-Inbar & G. Sharon (Eds.), Axe age: Acheulian tool-making from quarry to discard (pp. 7–44). London: Equinox.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F. (2017). How emotions are made: the secret life of the brain. New York: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlant, T., & Wynn, T. (2018). First sculpture: handaxe to figure stone. Chicago: Studio Blue.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binkofski, F., & Buccino, G. (2004). Motor functions of the Broca’s region. Brain and Language, 89(2), 362–369.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braun, D. R., Plummer, T. W., Ditchfield, P., Ferraro, J. V., Maina, D., Bishop, L. C., & Potts, R. (2008). Oldowan behavior and raw material transport: perspectives from the Kanjera formation. Journal of Archaeological Science, 35(8), 2329–2345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, N. B., Barner, D., Frank, M. C., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2014). Abacus: gesture in the mind, not the hands. In P. Bello, M. Guarini, M. McShane, & B. Scassellati (Eds.), Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society (Vol. 36, pp. 72–73). Austin: Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charlton, S. G., & Starkey, N. J. (2011). Driving without awareness: the effects of practice and automaticity on attention and driving. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 14(6), 456–471.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiang, T. (2002). Story of your life. In Stories of your life and others (pp. 91–145). Easthampton: Small Beer Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind: embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. J. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58(1), 7–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Specialization within the ventral stream: the case for the visual word form area. NeuroImage, 22, 466–476.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, J. (2014). The identity model: a theory to access visual display and hominin cognition within the Palaeolithic. In R. I. M. Dunbar, C. Gamble, & J. A. J. Gowlett (Eds.), Lucy to language: the benchmark papers (pp. 90–107). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coolidge, F. L., & Wynn, T. (2018). The rise of Homo sapiens: the evolution of modern thinking (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corbey, R., Jagich, A., Vaesen, K., & Collard, M. (2016). The Acheulean handaxe: more like a bird’s song than a Beatles’ tune? Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 25(1), 6–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Errico, F. (1998). Palaeolithic origins of artificial memory systems: an evolutionary perspective. In C. Renfrew & C. Scarre (Eds.), Cognition and material culture: the archaeology of symbolic storage (pp. 19–50). Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Preester, H., & Tsakiris, M. (2009). Body-extension versus body-incorporation: is there a need for a body-model? Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 8(3), 307–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2007). Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron, 56(2), 384–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Pegado, F., Braga, L. W., Ventura, P., Filho, G. N., Jobert, A., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Kolinsky, R., Morais, J., & Cohen, L. (2010). How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science, 330(6009), 1359–1364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Depew, D. J. (2003). Baldwin and his many effects. In B. Weber & D. Depew (Eds.), Evolution and learning: the Baldwin effect reconsidered (pp. 3–32). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diez-Martín, F., Yustos, P. S., Uribelarrea, D., Baquedano, E., Mark, D. F., Mabulla, A. Z. P., et al. (2015). The origin of the Acheulean: the 1.7 million-year-old site of FLK West, Olduvai Gorge (Tanzania). Scientific Reports, 5(17839), 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunnell, R. C. (1971). Systematics in prehistory. New York: Macmillan and Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falk, D. (1987). Hominid paleoneurology. Annual Review of Anthropology, 16(1), 13–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finn, J. K., Tregenza, T., & Norman, M. D. (2009). Defensive tool use in a coconut-carrying octopus. Current Biology, 19(23), R1069–R1070.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, W. T. (2000). The evolution of speech: a comparative review. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(7), 258–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, W. T. (2009). Fossil cues to the evolution of speech. In R. Botha & C. Knight (Eds.), The cradle of language (pp. 112–134). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, W. T. (2017). Empirical approaches to the study of language evolution. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(1), 3–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank, M. C., & Barner, D. (2012). Representing exact number visually using mental abacus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(1), 134–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallotti, R., & Mussi, M. (2017). Two Acheuleans, two humankinds: From 1.5 to 0.85 Ma at Melka Kunture (Upper Awash, Ethiopian highlands). Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 95, 1–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentilucci, M., & Corballis, M. C. (2006). From manual gesture to speech: a gradual transition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(7), 949–960.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1977). The theory of affordances. In R. Shaw & J. Bransford (Eds.), Perceiving, acting, and knowing: toward an ecological psychology (pp. 127–143). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Goren-Inbar, N., & Sharon, G. (2006). Invisible handaxes and visible Acheulian biface technology at Gesher Benot Ya’aqov, Israel. In N. Goren-Inbar & G. Sharon (Eds.), Axe age: Acheulian tool-making from quarry to discard (pp. 111–136). London: Equinox.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gowlett, J. A. J. (2006). The elements of design form in Acheulian bifaces: modes, modalities, rules and language. In N. Goren-Inbar & G. Sharon (Eds.), Axe age: Acheulian tool-making from quarry to discard (pp. 203–222). London: Equinox.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, K. R. L., & Schaller, G. B. (1964). Tool-using behavior of the California sea otter. Journal of Mammalogy, 45(2), 287–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hallos, J. (2005). “15 minutes of fame”: exploring the temporal dimension of Middle Pleistocene lithic technology. Journal of Human Evolution, 49(2), 155–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harmand, S., Lewis, J. E., Feibel, C. S., Lepre, C. J., Prat, S., Lenoble, A., Boës, X., Quinn, R. L., Brenet, M., Arroyo, A., Taylor, N., Clément, S., Daver, G., Brugal, J. P., Leakey, L., Mortlock, R. A., Wright, J. D., Lokorodi, S., Kirwa, C., Kent, D. V., & Roche, H. (2015). 3.3-million-year-old stone tools from Lomekwi 3, West Turkana, Kenya. Nature, 521(7552), 310–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heimann, K., Umilta, M. A., & Gallese, V. (2013). How the motor-cortex distinguishes among letters, unknown symbols and scribbles: a high density EEG study. Neuropsychologia, 51, 2833–2840.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herzlinger, G., Wynn, T., & Goren-Inbar, N. (2017). Expert cognition in the production sequence of Acheulian cleavers at Gesher Benot Ya’aqov, Israel: a lithic and cognitive analysis. PLoS One, 12(11), e0188337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higuchi, S., Chaminade, T., Imamizu, H., & Kawato, M. (2009). Shared neural correlates for language and tool use in Broca’s area. Neuroreport, 20(15), 1376–1381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodgson, D. (2000). Art, perception and information processing: an evolutionary perspective. Rock Art Research, 17(1), 3–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodgson, D. (2009). Evolution of the visual cortex and the emergence of symmetry in the Acheulean techno-complex. Comptes Rendus Palevol, 8(1), 93–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodgson, D. (2011). The first appearance of symmetry in the human lineage: where perception meets art. Symmetry, 3(1), 37–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodgson, D. (2015). The symmetry of Acheulean handaxes and cognitive evolution. Journal of Archaeological Science: Reports, 2, 204–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holloway, R. L. (1983). Human paleontological evidence relevant to language behavior. Human Neurobiology, 2(3), 105–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holloway, R. L., Sherwood, C. C., Hof, P. R., & Rilling, J. K. (2009). Evolution of the brain, in humans—paleoneurology. In L. R. Squire (Ed.), Encyclopedia of neuroscience (Vol. 4, pp. 1326–1334). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopkins, W. D., Meguerditchian, A., Coulon, O., Misiura, M., Pope, S., Mareno, M. C., & Schapiro, S. J. (2017). Motor skill for tool-use is associated with asymmetries in Broca’s area and the motor hand area of the precentral gyrus in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Behavioural Brain Research, 318, 71–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, G. R. (1996). Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows. Nature, 379(6562), 249–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (2005). Material anchors for conceptual blends. Journal of Pragmatics, 37(10), 1555–1577.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. D. (2013). Radically enactive cognition in our grasp. In Z. Radman (Ed.), The hand, an organ of the mind: what the manual tells the mental (pp. 227–252). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isaac, G. L. (1969). Studies of early culture in East Africa. World Archaeology, 1(1), 1–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isaac, G. L. (1976). Stages of cultural elaboration in the Pleistocene: possible archaeological indicators of the development of language capabilities. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 280(1), 275–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, K. H., & Engelhardt, L. (2012). The effects of handwriting experience on functional brain development in pre-literate children. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 1(1), 32–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, P. R. (1981). Experimental implement manufacture and use: a case study from Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania. In J. Z. Young, E. M. Jope, & K. P. Oakley (Eds.), The emergence of man (pp. 189–195). London: The Royal Society and the British Academy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Key, A. J. M., & Lycett, S. J. (2017). Influence of handaxe size and shape on cutting efficiency: a large-scale experiment and morphometric analysis. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 24(2), 514–541.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kihlstrom, J. F. (1987). The cognitive unconscious. Science, 237(4821), 1445–1452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kihlstrom, J. F. (1989). Cognition, unconscious processes. In G. Adelman (Ed.), Neuroscience year: supplement 1 to the encyclopedia of neuroscience (pp. 34–36). Boston: Birkhauser.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirsh, D. (2014). The importance of change and interactivity in creativity. Pragmatics & Cognition, 22(1), 5–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Konnikova, M. (2014). What’s lost as handwriting fades. The New York Times, p. D1. https://www.nytimes.com/2014/06/03/science/whats-lost-as-handwriting-fades.html

  • Koziol, L. F., Budding, D. E., & Chidekel, D. (2010). Adaptation, expertise, and giftedness: towards an understanding of cortical, subcortical, and cerebellar network contributions. Cerebellum, 9(4), 499–529.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: the embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lepre, C. J., Roche, H., Kent, D. V., Harmand, S., Quinn, R. L., Brugal, J.-P., et al. (2011). An earlier origin for the Acheulian. Nature, 477(7362), 82–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Longcamp, M., Zerbato-Poudou, M.-T., & Velay, J.-L. (2005). The influence of writing practice on letter recognition in preschool children: a comparison between handwriting and typing. Acta Psychologica, 119(1), 67–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovejoy, C. O. (1988). Evolution of human walking. Scientific American, 259(5), 82–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lycett, S. J. (2018). Cultural transmission from the last common ancestor to the Levallois reducers: what can we infer? In K. A. Overmann & F. L. Coolidge (Eds.), Squeezing minds from stones: cognitive archaeology and the evolution of the human mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Machin, A. J., Hosfield, R. T., & Mithen, S. J. (2007). Why are some handaxes symmetrical? Testing the influence of handaxe morphology on butchery effectiveness. Journal of Archaeological Science, 34(6), 883–893.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2008). Beads for a plastic mind: the “blind man’s stick” (BMS) hypothesis and the active nature of material culture. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 18(3), 401–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2010a). Knapping intentions and the marks of the mental. In L. Malafouris & C. Renfrew (Eds.), The cognitive life of things: recasting the boundaries of the mind (pp. 13–27). Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2010b). Metaplasticity and the human becoming: principles of neuroarchaeology. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 88, 49–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2013). How things shape the mind: a theory of material engagement. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2015). Metaplasticity and the primacy of material engagement. Time and Mind, 8(4), 351–371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maravita, A., & Iriki, A. (2004). Tools for the body (schema). Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(2), 79–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maravita, A., Husain, M., Clarke, K., & Driver, J. (2001). Reaching with a tool extends visual-tactile interactions into far space: evidence from cross-modal extinction. Neuropsychologia, 39(6), 580–585.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maravita, A., Spence, C., Kennett, S., & Driver, J. (2002). Tool-use changes multimodal spatial interactions between vision and touch in normal humans. Cognition, 83(2), B25–B34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, M. W., & Perston, Y. (2016). Experimental insights into the cognitive significance of early stone tools. PLoS One, 11(7), 1–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, P. A., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2014). The pen is mightier than the keyboard advantages of longhand over laptop note taking. Psychological Science, 25(6), 1159–1168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura, K., Kuo, W.-J., Pegado, F., Cohen, L., Tzeng, O. J.-L., & Dehaene, S. (2012). Universal brain systems for recognizing word shapes and handwriting gestures during reading. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(50), 20762–20767.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, H. J., Damerow, P., & Englund, R. K. (1993). Archaic bookkeeping: early writing and techniques of economic administration in the Ancient Near East. (P. Larsen, Trans.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D. R. (1994). The world on paper: the conceptual and cognitive implications of writing and reading. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D. R., & Cole, M. (Eds.). (2006). Technology, literacy, and the evolution of society: implications of the work of Jack Goody. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orban, G. A., Claeys, K., Nelissen, K., Smans, R., Sunaert, S., Todd, J. T., Wardak, C., Durand, J. B., & Vanduffel, W. (2006). Mapping the parietal cortex of human and non-human primates. Neuropsychologia, 44(13), 2647–2667.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overmann, K. A. (2016a). Beyond writing: the development of literacy in the Ancient Near East. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 26(2), 285–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overmann, K. A. (2016b). Number concepts are constructed through material engagement: a reply to Sutliff, Read, and Everett. Current Anthropology, 57(3), 352–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overmann, K. A. (2017). Thinking materially: cognition as extended and enacted. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 17(3–4), 381–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paddayya, K., Jhaldiyal, R., & Petraglia, M. D. (2006). The Acheulian quarry at Isampur, Lower Deccan, India. In N. Goren-Inbar & G. Sharon (Eds.), Axe age: Acheulian tool-making from quarry to discard (pp. 45–73). London: Equinox.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmeri, T. J. (2014). An exemplar of model-based cognitive neuroscience. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(2), 67–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pegado, F., Nakamura, K., & Hannagan, T. (2014). How does literacy break mirror invariance in the visual system? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penner-Wilger, M., Fast, L., LeFevre, J.-A., Smith-Chant, B. L., Skwarchuk, S.-L., Kamawar, D., & Bisanz, J. (2007). The foundations of numeracy: subitizing, finger gnosia, and fine motor ability. In D. S. McNamara & J. G. Trafton (Eds.), Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society (Vol. 29, pp. 1385–1390). Austin: Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pezzulo, G., Barsalou, L. W., Cangelosi, A., Fischer, M. H., McRae, K., & Spivey, M. J. (2011). The mechanics of embodiment: a dialog on embodiment and computational modeling. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. (2009). Is consciousness embodied? In P. Robbins & M. Aydede (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition (pp. 419–436). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putt, S. S., Woods, A. D., & Franciscus, R. G. (2014). The role of verbal interaction during experimental bifacial stone tool manufacture. Lithic Technology, 39(2), 96–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, M. B., & Parfitt, S. A. (Eds.). (1999). Boxgrove: a Middle Pleistocene hominid site at Eartham Quarry, Boxgrove, West Sussex. London: English Heritage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roux, F., Boetto, S., Sacko, O., Chollet, F., & Trémoulet, M. (2003). Writing, calculating, and finger recognition in the region of the angular gyrus: a cortical stimulation study of Gerstmann syndrome. Journal of Neurosurgery, 99(4), 716–727.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sampson, C. G. (2006). Acheulian quarries at hornfels outcrops in the Upper Karoo region of South Africa. In N. Goren-Inbar & G. Sharon (Eds.), Axe age: Acheulian tool-making from quarry to discard (pp. 75–110). London: Equinox.

  • Sanz, C. M., Call, J., & Morgan, D. B. (2009). Design complexity in termite-fishing tools of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Biology Letters: Animal Behaviour, 5, 293–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sapir, E. (1929). The status of linguistics as a science. Language, 5(4), 207–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schenker, N. M., Hopkins, W. D., Spocter, M. A., Garrison, A. R., Stimpson, C. D., Erwin, J. M., et al. (2009). Broca’s area homologue in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): probabilistic mapping, asymmetry, and comparison to humans. Cerebral Cortex, 20(3), 730–742.

    Google Scholar 

  • Senner, W. M. (1989). Theories and myths on the origins of writing: a historical overview. In W. M. Senner (Ed.), The origins of writing (pp. 1–26). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharon, G. (2008). The impact of raw material on Acheulian large flake production. Journal of Archaeological Science, 35(5), 1329–1344.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharon, G. (2009). Acheulian giant-core technology: a worldwide perspective. Current Anthropology, 50(3), 335–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherwood, C. C., Broadfield, D. C., Holloway, R. L., Gannon, P. J., & Hof, P. R. (2003). Variability of Broca’s area homologue in African great apes: implications for language evolution. The Anatomical Record, 271(2), 276–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shipton, C. (2010). Imitation and shared intentionality in the Acheulean. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 20(2), 197–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, B. C. (1999). Situatedness/embeddedness. In R. A. Wilson & F. C. Keil (Eds.), The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences (pp. 769–770). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. D. (2014). Prototypes, exemplars, and the natural history of categorization. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(2), 312–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. D., Zakrzewski, A. C., Johnson, J. M., & Valleau, J. C. (2016). Ecology, fitness, evolution: new perspectives on categorization. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(4), 266–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sülzenbrück, S., Hegele, M., Rinkenauer, G., & Heuer, H. (2011). The death of handwriting: secondary effects of frequent computer use on basic motor skills. Journal of Motor Behavior, 43(3), 247–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tobias, P. V. (1981). The emergence of man in Africa and beyond. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 292(1057), 43–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toth, N. P. (1985). The Oldowan reassessed: a close look at early stone artifacts. Journal of Archaeological Science, 12(2), 101–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tschentscher, N., Hauk, O., Fischer, M. H., & Pulvermüller, F. (2012). You can count on the motor cortex: finger counting habits modulate motor cortex activation evoked by numbers. NeuroImage, 59(4), 3139–3148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vandervert, L. R. (2009). The appearance of the child prodigy 10,000 years ago: an evolutionary and developmental explanation. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 30(1), 15–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vandervert, L. R., Schimpf, P. H., & Liu, H. (2007). How working memory and the cerebellum collaborate to produce creativity and innovation. Creativity Research Journal, 19(1), 1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Villeneuve, D. (2016). Arrival. United States: Paramount Pictures.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, A. C., Petersen, S. E., & Schlaggar, B. L. (2014). The VWFA: it’s not just for words anymore. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, R., & Horowitz, W. (2011). Writing science before the Greeks: a naturalistic analysis of the Babylonian astronomical treatise MUL. APIN. Leiden: Brill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wenban-Smith, F. F. (2004). Handaxe typology and Lower Paleolithic cultural development: ficrons, cleavers and two giant handaxes from Cuxton. Lithics: The Journal of the Lithic Studies Society, 25, 11–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whorf, B. L. (1940). Science and linguistics. Technology Review, 42(6), 229–231 247–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whorf, B. L. (1950). An American Indian model of the universe. International Journal of American Linguistics, 16(2), 67–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn, T. (1981). The intelligence of Oldowan hominids. Journal of Human Evolution, 10(7), 529–541.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn, T., & Gowlett, J. A. J. (2018). The handaxe reconsidered. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 27(1), 21–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn, T., Hernandez-Aguilar, R. A., Marchant, L. F., & McGrew, W. C. (2011). “An ape’s view of the Oldowan” revisited. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 20(5), 181–197.

  • Wynn, T., Overmann, K. A., Coolidge, F. L., & Janulis, K. (2017). Bootstrapping ordinal thinking. In T. Wynn & F. L. Coolidge (Eds.), Cognitive models in Palaeolithic archaeology (pp. 197–213). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Karenleigh A. Overmann.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Overmann, K.A., Wynn, T. Materiality and Human Cognition. J Archaeol Method Theory 26, 457–478 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10816-018-9378-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10816-018-9378-y

Keywords

  • Materiality
  • Writing
  • Stone tools
  • Cognitive evolution
  • Material engagement theory