Skip to main content
Log in

Specific Global–Local Visual Processing Abilities Mediate the Influence of Non-social Autistic-like Traits on Mental Rotation

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Inconsistent data are available on mental rotation performance in neurotypicals with high autistic-like traits. Here, we tested whether global–local visual processing abilities mediate the influence of specific autistic-like trait domains (social skill, attention switching, attention-to-detail, communication, and imagination) on mental rotation. Neurotypical participants (N = 128) underwent an assessment of autistic-like traits, a flanker task with hierarchical stimuli, and a mental rotation task. Path analysis showed that Reaction Times on the incongruent/local condition of the flanker task mediated the relationship between attention-to-detail and mental rotation accuracy. These findings indicate that a better capacity of ignoring incongruent global information to identify local information (reduced global interference) in persons with high non-social autistic-like traits, as attention-to-detail, facilitates mental rotation performance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Richler, J., Bisarya, D., Gurunathan, N., & Wheelwright, S. (2003). The systemising quotient (SQ): An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism and normal sex differences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B Biological Sciences, 358, 361–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bethell-Fox, C. E., & Shepard, R. N. (1988). Mental rotation: Effects of stimulus complexity and familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(1), 12–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, M., Daggar, R., & Collomosse, J. (2010). The relationship between systemising and mental rotation and the implications for the extreme male brain theory of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chamberlain, R., Van der Hallen, R., Huygelier, H., Van de Cruys, S., & Wagemans, J. (2017). Local-global processing bias is not a unitary individual difference in visual processing. Vision Research, 141, 247–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 233–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conson, M., Senese, V. P., Baiano, C., Zappullo, I., Warrier, V., The UNICAMPSY17 group, Salzano, S., Positano, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2020). The effects of autistic traits and academic degree on visuospatial abilities. Cognitive Processing, 21, 127–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, C. M., & Saucier, D. M. (2010). Mental rotation, targeting ability and Baron-Cohen’s Empathizing-Systemizing Theory of Sex Differences. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 712–716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cribb, S. J., Olaithe, M., Di Lorenzo, R., Dunlop, P. D., & Maybery, M. T. (2016). Embedded figures test performance in the broader autism phenotype: A meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 2924–2939.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, D. M., Warrier, V., Allison, C., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2018). Testing the Empathizing-Systemizing theory of sex differences and the Extreme Male Brain theory of autism in half a million people. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 115, 12152–12157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grinter, E. J., Van Beek, P. L., Maybery, M. T., & Badcock, D. R. (2009). Brief report: Visuospatial analysis and self-rated autistic-like traits. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 670–677.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grove, R., Baillie, A., Allison, C., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hoekstra, R. A. (2013). Empathizing, systemizing, and autistic traits: Latent structure in individuals with autism, their parents, and general population controls. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 22, 600–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoekstra, R. A., Bartels, M., Cath, D. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2008). Factor structure, reliability and criterion validity of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): A study in Dutch population and patient groups. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1555–1566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hollingshead, A. (1975). The four-factor index of social status. Unpublished manuscript. Yale University, Department of Sociology, New Haven, CT.

  • Hooven, C. K., Chabris, C. F., Ellison, P. T., & Kossyln, S. M. (2004). The relationship of male testosterone to components of mental rotation. Neuropsychologia, 42, 782–790.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (2004). LISREL 8.71 [Computer Software]. Scientific Software International Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1985). Cognitive coordinate systems: Accounts of mental rotation and individual differences in spatial ability. Psychological Review, 92, 137–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitazoe, N., Fujita, N., Izumoto, Y., Terada, S. I., & Hatakenaka, Y. (2017). Whether the Autism Spectrum Quotient consists of two different subgroups? Cluster analysis of the Autism Spectrum Quotient in general population. Autism, 21(3), 323–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (3rd ed.). Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • La Femina, F., Senese, V. P., Grossi, D., & Venuti, P. (2009). A battery for the assessment of visuo-spatial abilities involved in drawing tasks. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23, 691–714.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathôt, S., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 314–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., & Belleville, S. (1993). A study of perceptual analysis in a high-level autistic subject with exceptional graphic abilities. Brain and Cognition, 23, 279–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Belleville, S., & Ménard, E. (1999). Local bias in autistic subjects as evidenced by graphic tasks: Perceptual hierarchization or working memory deficit? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 743–755.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 27–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muth, A., Hönekopp, J., & Falter, C. M. (2014). Visuo-Spatial Performance in Autism: A Meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 3245–3263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees—Precedence of global features in visual-perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, C. J., Paton, B., Enticott, P. G., & Hohwy, J. (2015). “Subtypes” in the presentation of autistic traits in the general adult population. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 1291–1301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K. C., Swettenham, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Children with autism show local precedence in a divided attention task and global precedence in a selective attention task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 733–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pletzer, B. (2014). Sex-specific strategy use and global-local processing: A perspective toward integrating sex differences in cognition. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8, 425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putnick, D. L., & Bornstein, M. H. (2016). Measurement invariance conventions and reporting: The state of the art and future directions for psychological research. Developmental Review, 41, 71–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rilea, S. L. (2008). A lateralization of function approach to sex differences in spatial ability: A reexamination. Brain and Cognition, 67, 168–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, L. C., & Lamb, M. R. (1991). Neuropsychological contribution to theories of part/whole organization. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 299–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell-Smith, S. N., Maybery, M. T., Bayliss, D. M., & Sng, A. A. H. (2012). Support for a link between the local processing bias and social deficits in autism: An investigation of embedded figures test performance in non-clinical individuals. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 2420–2430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruta, L., Mazzone, D., Mazzone, L., Wheelwright, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2012). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient-Italian version: A cross-cultural confirmation of the broader autism phenotype. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 625–633.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Senese, V. P., Zappullo, I., Baiano, C., Zoccolotti, P., Monaco, M., & Conson, M. (2020). Identifying neuropsychological predictors of drawing skills in elementary school children. Child Neuropsychology, 26, 345–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, J. L., & Nonack, M. B. (2018). Gender differences in mental rotation strategy depend on degree of autistic traits. Autism Research, 11, 1024–1037.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics (3rd ed.). Harper Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trojano, L., Siciliano, M., Pedone, R., Cristinzio, C., & Grossi, D. (2015). Italian normative data for the Battery for Visuospatial Abilities (TERADIC). Neurological Sciences, 36, 1353–1361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Venuti, P., & Senese, V. P. (2007). Un questionario di autovalutazione degli stili parentali: uno studio su un campione italiano. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, XXXIV, 677–698.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, L., Mottron, L., Peng, D., Berthiaume, C., & Dawson, M. (2007). Local bias and local-to-global interference without global deficit: A robust finding in autism under various conditions of attention, exposure time, and visual angle. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 550–574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warrier, V., Toro, R., Won, H., Leblond, C. S., Cliquet, F., Delorme, R., De Witte, W., Bralten, J., Chakrabarti, B., Børglum, A. D., Grove, J., Poelmans, G., The 23andMe Research Team, Hinds, D. A., Bourgeron, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2019). Social and non-social autism symptoms and trait domains are genetically dissociable. Communications Biology, 2, 328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinbach, N., & Henik, A. (2001). Phasic alertness can modulate executive control by enhancing global processing of visual stimuli. Cognition, 121, 454–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yuille, J. C., & Steiger, J. H. (1982). Nonholistic processing in mental rotation: Some suggestive evidence. Perception & Psychophysics, 31, 201–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zapf, A. C., Glindemann, L. A., Vogeley, K., & Falter, C. M. (2015). Sex differences in mental rotation and how they add to the understanding of autism. PLoS One, 10, e0124628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zappullo, I., Conson, M., Zoccolotti, P., Trojano, L., & Senese, V. P. (2021). “Building blocks and drawing figures is not the same”: Neuropsychological bases of block design and Rey figure drawing in typically developing children. Child Neuropsychology, 27(3), 371–389. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2020.1862075

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

No funds were received for the study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

IZ: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, tasks administration, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing. VPS: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology. RM: Formal analysis, Project administration. MP: Software, Project administration. RC: Software, Data curation. GR: Data curation. MC: Conceptualization, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Massimiliano Conson.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in the present study were approved by the Local Ethics Committee and were in accordance with the ethical standards of the 1964 Helsinki declaration.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zappullo, I., Senese, V.P., Milo, R. et al. Specific Global–Local Visual Processing Abilities Mediate the Influence of Non-social Autistic-like Traits on Mental Rotation. J Autism Dev Disord 53, 80–88 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-05412-y

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-05412-y

Keywords

Navigation