Skip to main content
Log in

Advantage in Character Recognition Among Chinese Preschool Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined Chinese character recognition and its cognitive and linguistic correlates in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Forty-seven children with ASD and 51 IQ-matched typically developing (TD) children were tested on Chinese character recognition, rapid automatized naming, inhibitory control, digit span, IQ, vocabulary, phonological awareness, morphological awareness, and listening comprehension. Chinese children with ASD showed strong character recognition skills. Unlike TD children’s character recognition, which was correlated with all the measured cognitive and linguistic skills, character recognition of children with ASD was only significantly correlated with rapid automatized naming, inhibitory control, and phonological awareness. Our findings suggest that phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming may serve as important predictors for possible advantage in emergent literacy acquisition in Chinese children with ASD.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The standardized non-verbal IQ scores were established only for children over 5 years old. Children under five in this study therefore did not get the standardized score. The standardized non-verbal IQ scores for TD children and children with ASD over 5 years old were 98.5 and 74.9, respectively. Children under 5 years old consitituted 51% of the TD children and 39.1% of children with ASD.

  2. TEACCH refers to a framework that offers structured support for social communication using visual and/or written information to supplement verbal communication.

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., pp. 553–557). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R. C., Ku, Y. M., Li, W., Chen, X., Wu, X., & Shu, H. (2013). Learning to see the patterns in Chinese characters. Scientific Studies of Reading,17, 41–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Allison, C. (2008). The autism spectrum quotient: Children’s version (AQ-Child). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,38(7), 1230–1240. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-007-0504-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (2001). Metalinguistic aspects of bilingual processing. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics,21, 169–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castles, A., Crichton, A., & Prior, M. (2010). Developmental dissociations between lexical reading and comprehension: Evidence from two cases of hyperlexia. Cortex,46, 1238–1247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). (2018). Autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/autism/index.html

  • Child Assessment Service, Department of Health, Hong Kong SAR. (2006). Hong Kong cantonese oral language assessment scale. Hong Kong: Language Information Sciences Research Centre, City University of Hong Kong.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chung, K. K. H., & McBride-Chang, C. (2011). Executive functioning skills uniquely predict Chinese word reading. Journal of Educational Psychology,103, 909–921. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024744909.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Constantino, J. N., & Gruber, C. P. (2002). The social responsiveness scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dynia, J. M., Brock, M. E., Justice, L. M., & Kaderavek, J. N. (2017). Predictors of decoding for children with autism spectrum disorder in comparison to their peers. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders,37, 41–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2017.02.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • El Zein, F., Solis, M., Vaughn, S., & McCulley, L. (2014). Reading comprehension interventions for students with Autism Spectrum Disorders: A synthesis of research. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,44(6), 1303–1322. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1989-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Cambridge: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology,40, 177–190. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G. K., Aro, M., Liao, C.-H., & Parrila, R. (2016). Modeling the relationship between rapid automatized naming and literacy skills across languages varying in orthographic consistency. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,143, 48–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.10.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gliga, T., Bedford, R., Charman, T., Johnson, M. H., & The BASIS team. (2015). Enhanced visual search in infancy predicts emerging Autism symptoms. Current Biology,25, 1–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grigorenko, E. L., Volkmar, F., & Klin, A. (2003). Hyperlexia: Disability or superability? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,44, 1079–1091.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: detail focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,36(1), 5–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huemer, S. V., & Mann, V. (2010). A comprehensive profile of decoding and comprehension in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism Development Disorders,40, 485–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, M., Kirby, J., Cheng, L., Wade-Woolley, L., & Qiang, H. (2012a). Cognitive predictors of English reading achievement in Chinese English-immersion students. Reading Psychology,33(5), 423–447. https://doi.org/10.1080/02702711.2010.528663.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, H., Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Liu, H., & Peng, H. (2012b). Chinese children’s character recognition: Visuo-orthographic, phonological processing and morphological skills. Journal of Research in Reading,35(3), 287–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liao, C.-H., Georgiou, G. K., & Parrila, R. (2008). Rapid naming speed and Chinese character recognition. Reading and Writing,21, 231–253. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9071-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,30, 205–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,24, 659–685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., & Ho, C. (2000). Developmental issues in Chinese children’s character acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology,92, 50–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Zhou, A., Wat, C. P., & Wagner, R. K. (2003). Morphological awareness uniquely predicts young children’s Chinese character recognition. Journal of Educational Psychology,95, 743–751.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Tong, X. L., Shu, H., Wong, A. M. Y., Leung, K. W., & Tardif, T. (2008). Syllable, phoneme, and tone: Psycholinguistic units in early Chinese and English word recognition. Scientific Studies of Reading,12(2), 171–194. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888430801917290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McIntyre, N. S., Solari, E. J., Grimm, R. P. E., Lerro, L. E., Gonzales, J., & Mundy, P. C. (2017). A comprehensive examination of reading heterogeneity in students with high functioning autism: Distinct reading profiles and their relation to autism symptom severity. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,47, 1086–1101. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3029-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, L. E., Burke, J. D., Troyb, E., Knoch, K., Herlihy, L. E., & Fein, D. A. (2017). Preschool predictors of school-age academic achievement in autism spectrum disorder. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,31, 382–403. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2016.1225665.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murdaugh, D. L., Deshpande, H. D., & Kana, R. K. (2015). The impact of reading intervention on brain responses underlying language in children with autism. Autism Research,9, 141–154. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1503.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K. (1999). Reading skills in hyperlexia: A developmental perspective. Psychological Bulletin,125(3), 338–355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., Clarke, P., Wright, B., & Williams, C. (2006). Patterns of reading ability in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,36, 911–919. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0130-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, T. M., Macomber, D., Naples, A. J., Babits, T., Volkmar, F., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2007). Hyperlexia in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,37, 760–774.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norbury, C., & Nation, K. (2011). Understanding variability in reading comprehension in adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders: Interactions with language status and decoding skill. Scientific Studies of Reading,15, 191–210. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888431003623553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ostrolenk, A., Forgeot d’Arc, B., Jelenic, P., Samson, F., & Mottron, L. (2017). Hyperlexia: Systematic review, neurocognitive modelling, and outcome. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews,79, 134–149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.029.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pan, J., McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Liu, H., Zhang, Y., & Li, H. (2011). What is in the naming? A 5-year longitudinal study of early rapid naming and phonological sensitivity in relation to subsequent reading skills in both native Chinese and English as a second language. Journal of Educational Psychology,103, 897–908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pan, J., Song, S., Su, M. M., McBride, C., Liu, H. Y., Zhang, Y. P., et al. (2016). On the relationship between phonological awareness, morphological awareness and Chinese literacy skills: Evidence from an 8-year longitudinal study. Developmental Science,19, 982–991. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12356.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plaut, D. C. (2005). Connectionist approaches to reading. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 24–38). Malden: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1996). Manual for Raven’s standard progressive matrices. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sang, B., & Miao, X. (1990). The revision of trail norm of peabody picture vocabulary test revised (PPVT-R) in Shanghai proper. Psychological Science,5, 20–25. (In Chinese).

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition. Psychological Review,96, 523–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., Anderson, R. C., & Wu, N. (2000). Phonetic awareness: Knowledge of orthography–phonology relationships in the character acquisition of Chinese children. Journal of Educational Psychology,92, 56–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Wu, S., & Liu, H. Y. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia: Morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. Journal of Educational Psychology,98(1), 122–133. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., Peng, H., & McBride-Chang, C. (2008). Phonological awareness in young Chinese children. Developmental Science,11, 171–181. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00654.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M., & Frith, U. (1986). Comprehension in ‘‘hyperlexic’’ readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,42, 392–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Song, S., Georgiou, G. K., Su, M., & Shu, H. (2016). How well do phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming correlate with Chinese reading accuracy and fluency? A meta-analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading,20, 99–123. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2015.1088543.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tong, X., & McBride, C. (2014). Chinese children’s statistical learning of orthographic regularities: Positional constraints and character structure. Scientific Studies of Reading,18, 291–308. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2014.884098.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tong, X., & McBride-Chang, C. (2010a). Developmental models of learning to read Chinese words. Developmental Psychology,46(6), 1662. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020611.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tong, X., & McBride-Chang, C. (2010b). Chinese-English biscriptal reading: cognitive component skills across orthographies. Reading and Writing,23, 293–310. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9211-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tong, X. L., McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., & Wong, A. M. Y. (2009). Morphological awareness, orthographic knowledge, and spelling errors: Keys to understanding early Chinese literacy acquisition. Scientific Studies of Reading,13(5), 426–452. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888430903162910.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tong, X., Tong, X., & McBride, C. (2017). Unpacking the relation between morphological awareness and Chinese word reading: Levels of morphological awareness and vocabulary. Contemporary Educational Psychology,48, 167–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2016.07.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turkeltaub, P. E., Gareau, L., Flowers, D. L., Zeffiro, T. A., & Eden, G. F. (2003). Development of neural mechanisms for reading. Nature Neuroscience,6, 767–773.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vanegas, S. B., & Davidson, D. (2015). Investigating distinct and related contributions of weak central coherence, executive dysfunction, and systemizing theories to the cognitive profiles of children with Autism Spectrum Disorders and typically developing children. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders,11, 77–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.12.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, S., Frith, U., Milne, E., Rosen, S., Swettenham, J., & Ramus, F. (2006). A double dissociation between sensorimotor impairments and reading disability: A comparison of autistic and dyslexic children. Cognitive Neuropsychology,23, 748–761. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290500438607.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols,1, 297–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, H., & Wang, X. (1989). Standardization research on Raven’s standard progressive matrices in China. Acta Psychologica Sinica,2, 113–120. (in Chinese).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Tórth, D., Csépe, V., Reis, A., Faı´sca, L., et al. (2010). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: A cross-language investigation. Psychological Science,21, 551–559. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610363406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all children and their parents who participated in this study.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Social Science Fund of China [13CYY027] and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities [15000-31620003] to the first author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JZ and LY developed the study concept. All four authors contributed to the study design and SC analyzed the data. Testing and data collection were performed by JZ and LY. JZ drafted the manuscript, and XT provided critical revisions. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Jing Zhao or Li Yi.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zhao, J., Chen, S., Tong, X. et al. Advantage in Character Recognition Among Chinese Preschool Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 49, 4929–4940 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04202-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04202-x

Keywords

Navigation