Abstract
We explored whether children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) learn new nouns from overheard speech. Thirteen children (4–5 years) with ASD participated in an Addressed condition, in which they were directly taught a novel label (e.g., toma) for one of three novel objects, and an Overheard condition, in which the objects and label were presented in a conversation between two adults. In both conditions, children were then asked to identify the labeled object (e.g., “find the toma”). Children selected the target novel object at rates above chance in the Addressed condition, and of critical importance, they also did so in the Overheard condition. This suggests that, like TD children, children with ASD may learn from language that is not directed to them.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.


Notes
- 1.
One possible explanation for the lack of interpretable response on the second test is that children were confident in their response on the familiar word warm-up, but less so on the novel word trials and that they interpreted our second request as an indicator that their initial response was incorrect. Alternatively, perhaps children were still sufficiently engaged on the second test during the warm-up, but lost interest during the second test on the novel word trials. Akhtar et al. (2001) did not include a second test in their study with TD children.
References
Akechi, H., Senju, A., Kikuchi, Y., Tojo, Y., Osanai, H., & Hasegawa, T. (2011). Do children with ASD use referential gaze to learn the name of an object? An eye-tracking study. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(3), 1230–1242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2011.01.013.
Akhtar, N. (2005). The robustness of learning through overhearing. Developmental Science, 8, 199–209. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00406.x.
Akhtar, N., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2007). Joint attention and vocabulary development: A critical look. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1(3), 195–207. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2007.00014.x.
Akhtar, N., Jipson, J., & Callanan, M. A. (2001). Learning words through overhearing. Child Development, 72(2), 416–430. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00287.
Arunachalam, S., & Luyster, R. J. (2016). The integrity of lexical acquisition mechanisms in autism spectrum disorders: A research review. Autism Research, 9(8), 810–828. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1590.
Arunachalam, S., & Luyster, R. J. (under revision). Lexical development in young children with ASD: How ASD may affect intake from the input.
Baldwin, D. A. (1993a). Infants’ ability to consult the speaker for clues to word reference. Journal of Child Language, 20, 395–418. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900008345.
Baldwin, D. A. (1993b). Early referential understanding: Infants’ ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. Developmental Psychology, 29, 832–843. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.29.5.832.
Bang, J., & Nadig, A. (2015). Learning language in autism: Maternal linguistic input contributes to later vocabulary. Autism Research, 8(2), 214–223. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1440.
Bottema-Beutel, K. (2016). Associations between joint attention and language in autism spectrum disorder and typical development: A systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Autism Research, 9(10), 1021–1035. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1624.
Button, K. S., Ioannidis, J. P., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B. A., Flint, J., Robinson, E. S., & Munafò, M. R. (2013). Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(5), 365–376.
Charman, T., Drew, A., Baird, C., & Baird, G. (2003). Measuring early language development in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder using the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (Infant Form). Journal of Child Language, 30(1), 213. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000902005482.
Chita-Tegmark, M. (2016). Social attention in ASD: A review and meta-analysis of eye-tracking studies. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 48, 79–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2015.10.011.
Cristia, A., Dupoux, E., Gurven, M., & Stieglitz, J. (2017). Child-directed speech is infrequent in a forager-farmer population: A time allocation study. Child Development. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12974.
Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., et al. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 271. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.271.
Fenson, L., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Dale, P., Reznick, S., & Bates, E. (2006). The MacArthur communicative development inventories: User’s guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore: Brookes.
Fenson, L., Pethick, S., Renda, C., Cox, J. L., Dale, P. S., & Reznick, J. S. (2000). Short-form versions of the MacArthur communicative development inventories. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21(01), 95–116. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400001053.
Floor, P., & Akhtar, N. (2006). Can 18-month-old infants learn words by listening in on conversations? Infancy, 9(3), 327–339. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0903_4.
Franken, T. E., Lewis, C., & Malone, S. A. (2010). Brief report: Are children with autism proficient word learners? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(2), 255–259. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-0847-8.
Gampe, A., Liebal, K., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Eighteen-month-olds learn novel words through overhearing. First Language, 32(3), 385–397. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723711433584.
Hani, H. B., Gonzalez-Barrero, A. M., & Nadig, A. S. (2013). Children’s referential understanding of novel words and parent labeling behaviors: similarities across children with and without autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Language, 40(5), 971–1002. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000912000426.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children (Vol. xxiii). Baltimore: Paul H Brookes Publishing.
Hedge, C., Powell, G., & Sumner, P. (2017). The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behavior Research Methods. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0935-1.
Jaswal, V. K., & Markman, E. M. (2001). Learning proper and common names in inferential versus ostensive contexts. Child Development, 72(3), 768–786. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00314.
Korhonen, V., Kärnä, E., & Räty, H. (2014). Autism spectrum disorder and impaired joint attention: A review of joint attention research from the past decade. Nordic Psychology, 66(2), 94–107. https://doi.org/10.1080/19012276.2014.921577.
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. L. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule, second edition (ADOS-2) manual (part I): Modules 1–4. Torrance: Western Psychological Services.
Luyster, R., & Lord, C. (2009). Word learning in children with autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Psychology, 45(6), 1774–1786. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016223.
Luyster, R. J., Kadlec, M. B., Carter, A., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2008). Language assessment and development in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(8), 1426–1438. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-007-0510-1.
Magiati, I., Tay, X. W., & Howlin, P. (2014). Cognitive, language, social and behavioural outcomes in adults with autism spectrum disorders: A systematic review of longitudinal follow-up studies in adulthood. Clinical Psychology Review, 34(1), 73–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2013.11.002.
Mawhood, L., Howlin, P., & Rutter, M. (2000). Autism and developmental receptive language disorder—a comparative follow-up in early adult life. I: Cognitive and language outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 41(5), 547–559. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00642.
Naigles, L. R., Cheng, M., Rattanasone, N. X., Tek, S., Khetrapal, N., Fein, D., et al. (2016). “You’re telling me!” The prevalence and predictors of pronoun reversals in children with autism spectrum disorders and typical development. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 27, 11–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2016.03.008.
O’Neill, D. (2009). Language Use Inventory: An assessment of young children’s pragmatic language development for 18-to 47-month-old children [manual]. Waterloo, ON: Knowledge in Development.
O’Neill, D. K. (2007). The language use inventory for young children: A parent-report measure of pragmatic language development for 18-to 47-month-old children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(1), 214–228. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/017).
Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. B. (1984). Language acquisition and socializations: Three developmental stories and their implications. In R. A. Shweder & R. A. LeVine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self and emotion (pp. 276–320). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Oshima-Takane, Y. (1988). Children learn from speech not addressed to them: The case of personal pronouns. Journal of Child Language, 15(01), 95–108. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900012071.
Oshima-Takane, Y., & Benaroya, S. (1989). An alternative view of pronominal errors in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19(1), 73–85. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02212719.
Oshima-Takane, Y., Goodz, E., & Derevensky, J. L. (1996). Birth order effects on early language development: Do secondborn children learn from overheard speech? Child Development, 67(2), 621–634. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01755.x.
Parish-Morris, J., Hennon, E. A., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2007). Children with autism illuminate the role of social intention in word learning. Child Development, 78(4), 1265–1287. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01065.x.
Pepperberg, I. M., & Sherman, D. (2000). Proposed use of two-part interactive modeling as a means to increase functional skills in children with a variety of disabilities. Teaching and Learning in Medicine, 12(4), 213–220. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328015TLM1204_10.
Pepperberg, I. M., & Sherman, D. V. (2002). Use of two-trainer interactive modeling as a potential means to engender social behavior in children with various disabilities. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 15(2), 138–153. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/8kv8t6qg.
Pierce, K., Glad, K. S., & Schreibman, L. (1997). Social perception in children with autism: An attentional deficit? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(3), 265–282. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025898314332.
Sandbank, M., & Yoder, P. (2016). The association between parental mean length of utterance and language outcomes in children with disabilities: A correlational meta-analysis. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 25(2), 240–251. https://doi.org/10.1044/2015_AJSLP-15-0003.
Shneidman, L. A., Buresh, J. S., Shimpi, P. M., Knight-Schwarz, J., & Woodward, A. L. (2009). Social experience, social attention and word learning in an overhearing paradigm. Language Learning and Development, 5(4), 266–281. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2015.10.011.
Shneidman, L. A., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012). Language input and acquisition in a Mayan village: How important is directed speech? Developmental Science, 15(5), 659–673. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01168.x.
Sigman, M., Ruskin, E., Arbelle, S., Corona, R., Dissanayake, C., Espinosa, M., et al. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down syndrome, and developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64, 1–114.
Snow, C. E., & Ferguson, C. A. (Eds.). (1977). Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Steiger, J. H. (1980). Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 87, 245–251. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.87.2.245.
Venter, A., Lord, C., & Schopler, E. (1992). A follow-up study of high-functioning autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 489–507. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb00887.x.
Warren, S. F., Gilkerson, J., Richards, J. A., Oller, D. K., Xu, D., Yapanel, U., et al. (2010). What automated vocal analysis reveals about the vocal production and language learning environment of young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(5), 555–569. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-0902-5.
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2143–2152. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613488145.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by an Emerson College Faculty Advancement Fund Grant and an American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Advancing Academic-Research Careers Award to the first author, and NIH K01DC013306 to the second author. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. We would like to express our gratitude to Molly Atkinson, Julia Bird, Nicole Coughlin, Marisa Curtis, Jessica Ghofrani, Katelyn Li, Amanda Netburn, Rea Ramos, Alicia Reifler, Madeline Saunders and Nicholas Souter for their help with design, recruitment, data collection and manuscript preparation. We would also like to thank the children and families who contributed their time and effort to make this project possible.
Author information
Affiliations
Contributions
RJL and SA contributed equally to this work.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
Rhiannon Luyster is an author on the ADOS-2 and receives royalties from sales. S. Arunachalam declares that she has no conflict of interest.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Luyster, R.J., Arunachalam, S. Brief Report: Learning Language Through Overhearing in Children with ASD. J Autism Dev Disord 50, 2616–2624 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3672-0
Published:
Issue Date:
Keywords
- Autism spectrum disorder
- Word learning
- Language