Skip to main content
Log in

Emotional Processing in Autism Spectrum Disorders: Effects of Age, Emotional Valence, and Social Engagement on Emotional Language Use

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) show deficits in reporting others’ emotions (Lartseva et al. in Front Hum Neurosci 8:991, 2015) and in deriving meaning in social contexts (Klin et al. in Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders, Wiley, Hoboken, 2005). However, researchers often use stimuli that conflate salient emotional and social information. Using a matched-pairs design, the impact of emotional and social information on emotional language in pre-school and school-age children, with and without ASD, was assessed with a picture description task comprising rated stimuli from the Pictures with Social Contexts and Emotional Scenes database (Teh et al. in Behav Res Methods, https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0947-x, 2017). Results showed both groups with ASD produced fewer emotional terms than typically developing children, but the effects were moderated by valence, social engagement, and age. Implications for theory and clinical practice are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Studies on internal state language use in ASD using story-narratives are few at present, and mixed findings have been reported, with some reporting reduced frequency and diversity of internal state language used by children with ASD compared to control groups (Rumpf et al. 2012; Siller et al. 2014), and others reporting no significant difference between groups (Canfield et al. 2016; Capps et al. 2000; Kristen et al. 2015). However, it is beyond the scope of the present study to discuss findings on internal state language, as they do not clearly examine emotional language skills.

  2. Some of the participants with ASD, and all the typically developing participants, were likely to be from bilingual homes and/or learning a second language in school. In mainstream primary schools in Singapore, all subjects are taught in English, except for the Mother Tongue subject, which is in Mandarin Chinese, Malay or Tamil. However, students with special educational needs, such as children with ASD, are sometimes exempt from the Mother Tongue requirement upon request by parents and approval by the Ministry of Education.

  3. We did not statistically control for language and theory-of-mind skills by entering them as covariates in our analyses because the assumption of equal values of the covariates in all participant groups is not met in this sample (Schneider et al. 2015).

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental diseases (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Balconi, M., Amenta, S., & Ferrari, C. (2012). Emotional decoding in facial expression, scripts and videos: A comparison between normal, autistic and Asperger children. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 193–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Begeer, S., Koot, H. M., Rieffe, C., Terwogt, M. M., & Stegge, H. (2008). Emotional competence in children with autism: Diagnostic criteria and empirical evidence. Developmental Review, 28, 342–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Itzchak, E., Abutbul, S., Bela, H., Shai, T., & Zachor, D. A. (2016). Understanding one’s own emotions in cognitively-able preadolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 2363–2371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Yosef, D., Anaki, D., & Golan, O. (2017). Context processing in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: How complex could it be? Autism Research, 10, 520–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal states: The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 18, 906–921.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, R., Biotti, F., Bird, G., & Cook, R. (2017). Typical integration of emotion cues from bodies and faces in autism spectrum disorder. Cognition, 165, 82–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Canfield, A. R., Eigsti, I. M., de Marchena, A., & Fein, D. (2016). Story goodness in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and in optimal outcomes from ASD. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Capps, L., Losh, M., & Thurber, C. (2000). “The frog ate the bug and made his mouth sad”: Narrative competence in children with autism. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 193–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carrow-Woolfolk, E. (1999). Comprehensive assessment of spoken language. Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charles, S. T., Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and emotional memory: The forgettable nature of negative images for older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 310–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E. S., & Schultz, R. T. (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 231–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Constantino, J. N. (2012). The Social Responsiveness Scale (2nd ed.). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diehl, J. J., Bennetto, L., & Young, E. C. (2006). Story recall and narrative coherence of high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 83–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doody, J. P., & Bull, P. (2013). Evidence for impaired verbal identification but intact nonverbal recognition of fearful body postures in Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 1652–1661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression and emotion. American Psychologist, 48, 384–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, N., & Happé, F. (2005). A training study of theory of mind and executive function in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 757–771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forgeot d’Arc, B., Ramus, F., Lefebvre, A., Brottier, D., Zalla, T., Moukawane, S., … Delorme, R. (2016). Atypical social judgment and sensitivity to perceptual cues in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 1574–1581.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581–586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 1337–1345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goren, C., Sarty, M., & Wu, P. (1975). Visual following and pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. Pediatrics, 56, 544–549.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2014). Annual research review: Towards a developmental neuroscience of atypical social cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 553–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, E. L. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 26–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. E. (1992). Basic emotions, relations among emotions, and emotion-cognition relations. Psychological Review, 99, 561–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson, M., Mecklinger, A., & Treese, A. (2004). Recognition memory for emotional and neutral faces: An event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1840–1853.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kauschke, C., van der Beek, B., & Kamp-Becker, I. (2016). Narratives of girls and boys with autism spectrum disorders: Gender differences in narrative competence and internal state language. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 840–852.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kavanaugh, R., Eizenman, D., & Harris, P. (1997). Young children’s understanding of pretense expressions of independent agency. Developmental Psychology, 33, 764–770.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809–816.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R. T., & Volkmar, F. R. (2005). The enactive mind—from actions to cognition: Lessons from autism. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (Vol. 1, 3rd ed., pp. 682–703). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kristen, S., Vuori, M., & Sodian, B. (2015). “I love the cute caterpillar!” autistic children’s production of internal state language across contexts and relations to joint attention and theory of mind. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 12, 22–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lartseva, A., Dijkstra, T., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2015). Emotional language processing in autism spectrum disorders: A systematic review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 991.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R. S. (1982). Thoughts on the relations between emotion and cognition. American Psychologist, 37, 1019–1024.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lemerise, E. A., & Arsenio, W. F. (2000). An integrated model of emotion processes and cognition in social information processing. Child Development, 71, 107–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule: ADOS-2 (2nd ed.). Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Losh, M., & Capps, L. (2006). Understanding of emotional experience in autism: Insights from the personal accounts of high-functioning children with autism. Developmental Psychology, 42, 809–818.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (1998). Autism as a disorder of complex information processing. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 4, 129–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nuske, H. J., Vivanti, G., & Dissanayake, C. (2013). Are emotion impairments unique to, universal, or specific in autism spectrum disorder? A comprehensive review. Cognition and Emotion, 27, 1042–1061.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ochsner, K. N. (2008). The social-emotional processing stream: Five core constructs and their translational potential for schizophrenia and beyond. Biological Psychiatry, 64, 48–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ohman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466–478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olsson, A., & Ochsner, K. N. (2008). The role of social cognition in emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 65–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C. C., Wellman, H. M., & Slaughter, V. (2012). The mind behind the message: Advancing theory-of-mind scales for typically developing children, and those with deafness, autism, or Asperger syndrome. Child Development, 83, 469–485.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Pons, F., Lawson, J., Harris, P. L., & de Rosnay, M. (2003). Individual differences in children’s emotion understanding: Effects of age and language. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 44, 347–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pratt, C., & Bryant, P. (1990). Young children understand that looking leads to knowing (so long as they are looking into a single barrel). Child Development, 61, 973–982.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1995). Raven’s colored progressive matrices. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1998). Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rickard Liow, S., & Sze, W. (2009). Bilingual Language Assessment Battery (BLAB). (unpublished).

  • Rieffe, C., Terwogt, M. M., & Kotronopoulou, K. (2007). Awareness of single and multiple emotions in high-functioning children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 455–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubo, M., & Gamer, M. (2018). Social content and emotional valence modulate gaze fixations in dynamic scenes. Scientific Reports, 8, 3804. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-22127-w.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rumpf, A. L., Kamp-Becker, I., Becker, K., & Kauschke, C. (2012). Narrative competence and internal state language of children with Asperger Syndrome and ADHD. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33, 1395–1407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, B. A., Avivi-Reich, M., & Mozuraitis, M. (2015). A cautionary note on the ues of the analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) in classification designs with and without within-subject factors. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 474.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1061–1086.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siller, M., Swanson, M. R., Serlin, G., & Teachworth, A. (2014). Internal state language in the storybook narratives of children with and without autism spectrum disorder: Investigating relations to theory of mind abilities. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8, 589–596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steele, S., Joseph, R. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). Brief report: Developmental change in theory of mind abilities in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 461–467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, K., Zaitchik, D., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (1994). Preschoolers can attribute second-order beliefs. Developmental Psychology, 30, 395–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (1992). Autistic children’s talk about psychological states: Deficits in the early acquisition of a theory of mind. Child Development, 63, 161–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). Understanding the language and communicative impairments in autism. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 23, 185–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teh, E. J., Yap, M. J., & Rickard Liow, S. J. (2017). PiSCES: Pictures with social context and emotional scenes with norms for emotional valence, intensity and social engagement. Behavior Research Methods. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0947-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uljarevic, M., & Hamilton, A. (2013). Recognition of emotions in autism: A formal meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 1517–1526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., & Woolley, J. D. (1990). From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology. Cognition, 35, 245–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willig, C. (2001). Introducing qualitative research in psychology: Adventures in theory and method. London: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining functions of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziv, Y., Hadad, B. S., & Khateeb, Y. (2014). Social information processing in preschool children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 846–859.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all children and parents who participated in this study. We are also grateful to all participating schools and the Ministry of Education of Singapore, for their kind support of this research. Lastly, we thank our research assistants, Oh Jing Jing, Lee Jia Wen, Tan Lin Er and Joy Teo. This paper is based on part of a doctoral study completed by E. J. Teh, under the supervision of S. J. Rickard Liow and M. J. Yap, and portions of this paper were previously presented at the National Psychology Graduate Students Conference 2017, Singapore, and the International Society for Autism Research Conference 2018, Rotterdam, The Netherlands. The study was partially supported by research grants from the Singapore Children’s Society and the Faculty of Arts & Social Sciences, National University of Singapore.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors conceptualized and designed the study. EJT coordinated the collection of the data, performed the statistical analyses, and drafted the manuscript; MJY and SJRL provided feedback on the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elizabeth J. Teh.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interests.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from parents of all individual child participants included in the study, and verbal consent was obtained from all individual child participants included in the study.

Appendix

Appendix

List of Stimulus Pictures

See Table 5.

Table 5 The picture numbers below refer to numbering in the PiSCES Database (Teh et al. 2017)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Teh, E.J., Yap, M.J. & Rickard Liow, S.J. Emotional Processing in Autism Spectrum Disorders: Effects of Age, Emotional Valence, and Social Engagement on Emotional Language Use. J Autism Dev Disord 48, 4138–4154 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3659-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3659-x

Keywords

Navigation