Skip to main content
Log in

Exploring the Components of Advanced Theory of Mind in Autism Spectrum Disorder

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Cite this article

Abstract

Performance of a group of 35 youth and adults with High-Functioning Autism (HFA) was compared with a typical developing (TD) group on three Advanced Theory of Mind tests. The distinction between the social-cognitive and social-perceptual components of Theory of Mind was also explored. The HFA group had more difficulties in all tasks. Performance on the two social-cognitive tests was highly correlated in the HFA group, but these were not related with the social-perceptual component. These results suggest that the youth with HFA have difficulties on all the components of social knowledge but may be using different underlying cognitive abilities depending on the nature of the task.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abell, F., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2000). Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cognitive Development, 15, 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adolphs, R., Gosselin, F., Buchanan, T. W., Tranel, D., Schyns, P., & Damasio, A. R. (2005). A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala damage. Nature, 433(7021), 68–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association (2002). DSM-IV-TR: Manual diagnóstico y estadístico de los trastornos mentales. Barcelona: Masson.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th edn.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, A. K., Christoff, K., Panitz, D., DeRosa, E., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2003). Neural correlates of automatic processing of threat facial signals. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 5627–5633.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ashwin, C., Wheelwright, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Finding a face in the crowd: Testing the anger superiority effect in autism. Brain and Cognition, 61, 78–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bach, E., Ropar, D., & Mitchell, P. (2007). Do the eyes have it? Inferring mental states from animated faces in autism. Child Development, 78, 397–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Roberts, M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 813–822.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Bullmore, E. T., Wheelwright, S., Ashwin, C., & Williams, S. C. (2000). The amygdala theory of autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews, 24(3), 355–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Wheelwright, S., Bullmore, E., Brammer, M., & Simmons, A. (1999). Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: An fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 11, 1891–1898.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. (2000). Understanding other minds. Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 241–252.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boucher, J., Mayes, A., Cowell, P., Broks, P., Farrant, A., & Roberts, N. (2005). A combined clinical, neuropsychological and neuroanatomical study of adults with HFA. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 10, 165–214.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bowler, D. M. (1992). `Theory of mind’ in Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 877–893.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brent, E., Rios, P., Happé, F., & Charman, T. (2004). Performance of children with autism spectrum disorder on advanced theory of mind tasks. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 8, 283–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buitelaar, J. K., & Van Der Wees, M. (1997). Are deficits in the decoding of affective cues and in mentalizing abilities independent? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 539–556.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Capps, L., Kasari, C., Yirmiya, N., & Sigman, M. (1993). Parental perception of emotional expressiveness in children with autism. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 475–484.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, L. (2015). Theory of mind in utterance interpretation: The case from clinical pragmatics. Frontiers in Psychology, 26(6), 1286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denett, D. C. (1978). Beliefs about beliefs. Behaviour and Brain Sciences, 4, 568–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duval, C., Piolino, P., Bejanin, A., Eustache, F., & Desgranges, B. (2011). Age effects on different components of theory of mind. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(3), 627–642.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenmajer, R., Prior, M., Leekam, S., Wing, L., Gould, J., Welham, M., & Ong, B. (1996). Comparison of clinical symptoms in autism and Asperger’s disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35(11), 1523–1531.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ghaziuddin, M., & Mountain-Kimchi, K. J. (2004). Defining the intellectual profile of Asperger syndrome: Comparison with high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 279–284.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gould, J., & Ashton-Smith, J. (2011). Missed diagnosis or misdiagnosis? Girls and women on the autism spectrum. Good Autism Practice (GAP), 12(1), 34–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, C., Lough, S., Stone, V., Erzinclioglu, S., Martin, L., Baron-Cohen S., & Hodges J. R. (2002). Theory of mind in patients with frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: theoretical and practical implications. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 125(4), 752–764.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, J. B., Klin, A., Carter, A. S. & Volkmar, F. R. (2000). Verbal bias in recognition of facial emotions in children with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 369–379.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 129–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (1994). Theory of mind in autism. In E. Shopler & G. Mesibov. (Eds.), Learning and cognition in autism. New Tork: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heavey, L., Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (2000). The awkward moments test: a naturalistic measure of social understanding in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 225–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). The strange stories test: A replication with high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(5), 395–406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaland, N., Callesen, K., Møller-Nielsen, A., Mortensen, E. L., & Smith, L. (2008). Performance of children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism on advanced theory of mind tasks. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1112–1123.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaland, N., Møller-Nielsen, A., Callesen, K., Mortensen, E. L., Gottlieb, D., & Smith, L. (2002). An ‘advanced’ test of theory of mind: Evidence from children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 517–528.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, E., Kyeong, S., Cheon, K., Park, B., Oh, M. K., Chun, J., et al. (2016). Neural responses to affective and cognitive theory of mind in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Neuroscience Letters, 621, 117–125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A. (2000). Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher—functioning autism and Asperger syndrome: The social attribution task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 831–846.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. (2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: Lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of The Royal Society B Biological Sciences, 358(1430), 345–360.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leekam, S. R., López, B., & Moore, C. (2000). Attention and joint attention in preschool children with autism. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 261–273.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism diagnostic observation schedule. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Macintosh, K. E. & Dissanayake, C. (2004). Annotation: The similarities and differences between autistic disorder and Asperger’s disorder: A review of the empirical evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 45(3), 421–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazza, M., Pino, M. C., Mariano, M., Tempesta, D., Ferrara, M., De Berardis, D., et al. (2014). Affective and cognitive empathy in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 791.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Medina-Pradas, C., Navarro, J. B., Álvarez-Moya, E. M., Grau, A., & Obiols, J. E. (2012). Emotional theory of mind in eating disorders. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 12(2), 189–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J., Frith, U., Leslie, A. M. (1991). The cognitive basis of a biological disorder autism. Trends in Neurosciences, 14, 434–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mouga, S., Café, C., Almeida, J., Marques, C., Duque, F., & Oliveira, G. (2016). Intellectual profiles in the autism spectrum and other neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(9), 2940–2955.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., & McMahon Griffith, E. (2000). Neuropsychological function and the external validity of Asperger syndrome. In A. Klin, F. R. Volkmar & S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger syndrome (pp. 72–96). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pelphrey, K. A., Sasson, N. J., Reznick, J. S., Paul, G., Goldman, B. D., & Piven, J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 249–261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., & WImmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that...” attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39(3), 437–471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pileggi, L. A., Malcolm-Smith, S., & Solms, M. (2015). Investigating the role of social-affective attachment processes in cradling bias: the absence of cradling bias in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Laterality, 20, 154–170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ponnet, K., Roeyers, H., Buysse, A., De Clercq, A., & Van der Heyden, E. (2004). Advanced mind-reading in adults with Asperger syndrome. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 8, 249–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a “theory of mind’’? Behaviour and Brain Sciences, 4, 515–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pruett, J. R., Kandala, S., Petersen, S. E., & Povinelli, D. J. (2015). Brief report: Theory of mind, relational reasoning, and social responsiveness in children with and without autism: Demonstration of feasibility for a larger scale study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 2243–2251.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Roeyers, H., Buysse, A., Ponnet, K., & Pichal, B. (2001). Advancing advanced mind-reading tests: Empathic accuracy in adults with a pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of Child Psychiatry and Psychology, 42, 271–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutherford, M. D., & McIntosh, D. (2007). Rules versus Prototype matching: Strategies of perception of emotional facial expressions in the autism spectrum. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 187–196.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegal, M., & Varley, R. (2006). Aphasia, language, and theory of mind. Social Neuroscience, 1(3–4), 167–174.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spek, A., Scholte, E., & Van Berckelaer-Onnes, I. (2010). Theory of mind in adults with HFA and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(3), 280–289.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 640–656.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (1993). What language reveals about the understanding of mind in children with autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds—Perspectives from autism (pp. 138–157). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). A re-examination of the theory of mind hypothesis of autism. In J. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya&amp & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), Development in autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 173–193). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (1994). A second look at second-order belief attribution in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 577–586.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (2000). A componential view of theory of mind: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognition, 76, 59–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1999). Escala de Inteligencia Wechsler para Adultos (WAIS-III). Madrid: TEA Ediciones, S.A.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (2005). Escala de Inteligencia Wechsler para niños (WISC-IV). Madrid: TEA Ediciones, S.A.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H. & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs. Cognitions, 13, 103–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wing, L. (2005). Reflections on opening Pandora’s box. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(2), 197–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zalla, T., Sav, A., Stopin, A., Leboyer, M., & Ahade, S. (2008). Faux pas detection and intentional action in Asperger syndrome. A replication on a French sample. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(2), 373–382.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, D., Ownsworth, T., O’Donovan, A., Roberts, J., & Gullo, M. J. (2016). Independence of hot and cold executive function deficits in high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 10–24.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by grant from Fondo Investigación Sanitaria (FIS PI081217). We express our sincere thanks to the participants and their families. We also gratefully acknowledge to a large number of professionals for their help with recruiting and testing the sample.

Author Contributions

CP conceived of the study, participated in its design, acquisition of data and performed the measurement, interpretation of the data and drafted the manuscript. EP participated in the design, helped to draft the manuscript and critical revision of the manuscript. JBN participated in the design, performed the statistical analysis and interpretation of the data. MP conceived of the study, participated in its design and critical revision of the manuscript. JEO conceived of the study, participated in its design and coordination and critical revision of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to J. E. Obiols.

Ethics declarations

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and /or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pedreño, C., Pousa, E., Navarro, J.B. et al. Exploring the Components of Advanced Theory of Mind in Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 47, 2401–2409 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3156-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3156-7

Keywords

Navigation