Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 47, Issue 6, pp 1791–1805 | Cite as

Attempting to “Increase Intake from the Input”: Attention and Word Learning in Children with Autism

  • Elena J. Tenenbaum
  • Dima Amso
  • Giulia Righi
  • Stephen J. Sheinkopf
Original Paper

Abstract

Previous work has demonstrated that social attention is related to early language abilities. We explored whether we can facilitate word learning among children with autism by directing attention to areas of the scene that have been demonstrated as relevant for successful word learning. We tracked eye movements to faces and objects while children watched videos of a woman teaching them new words. Test trials measured participants’ recognition of these novel word-object pairings. Results indicate that for children with autism and typically developing children, pointing to the speaker’s mouth while labeling a novel object impaired performance, likely because it distracted participants from the target object. In contrast, for children with autism, holding the object close to the speaker’s mouth improved performance.

Keywords

Autism Eye-tracking Word-learning Attention to faces Joint attention 

Notes

Acknowledgments

Special thanks to Madeline Mahowald and Emily Davis for their assistance with the stimuli and to the participant families.

Funding

This research was supported by grants from the Autism Science Foundation (Grant No.11-1013), Autism Speaks (Grant No. 7897), and the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) (Grant No. 5T32MH019927-24).

Author contribution

ET helped in conception of the study, participated in its design, coordinated data collection, performed the statistical analysis, and drafted the manuscript; DA participated in the design and interpretation of the data; GR helped in conception and design of the study, and participated in interpretation of the data; SS participated in the design of the study, data collection, and interpretation of the data. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

References

  1. Akechi, H., Kikuchi, Y., Tojo, Y., Osanai, H., & Hasegawa, T. (2013). Brief report: pointing cues facilitate word learning in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(1), 230–235. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1555-3.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Akechi, H., Senju, A., Kikuchi, Y., Tojo, Y., Osanai, H., & Hasegawa, T. (2011). Do children with ASD use referential gaze to learn the name of an object? An eye-tracking study. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(3), 1230–1242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Aldaqre, I., Paulus, M., & Sodian, B. (2015). Referential gaze and word learning in adults with autism. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 19(8), 944–955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Arunachalam, S., & Luyster, R. J. (2015). The integrity of lexical acquisition mechanisms in autism spectrum disorders: A research review. Autism Research, 9(8), 810–828.Google Scholar
  5. Baldwin, D. A. (1991). Infants’ contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development, 62(5), 875–890.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Baldwin, D. A. (1993). Early referential understanding: Infants’ ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. Developmental Psychology, 29(5), 832–843.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Baldwin, D. A., Markman, E. M., Bill, B., Desjardins, R. N., Irwin, J. M., & Tidball, G. (1996). Infants’ reliance on a social criterion for establishing word-object relations. Child Development, 67(6), 3135–3153.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Baldwin, D. A., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Links between social understanding and early word learning: Challenges to current accounts. Social Development, 10(3), 309–329 doi: 10.1111/1467-9507.00168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Baron-Cohen, S., Baldwin, D. A., & Crowson, M. (1997). Do children with autism use the speaker’s direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Child Development, 68(1), 48–57.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Bayley, N. (2005). Bayley scales of infant development, 3rd ed.. San Antonio: PsychCorp.Google Scholar
  11. Bloom, P., & Markson, L. (1998). Capacities underlying word learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(2), 67–73.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Bono, M. A., Daley, L. T., & Sigman, M. (2004). Relations among joint attention, amount of intervention and language gain in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(5), 495–505.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2005). The development of gaze following and its relation to language. Developmental Science, 8(6), 535–543. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00445.x.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: A longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. Journal of Child Language, 35(1), 207–220.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society of Research in Child Development, 63, i–vi, 1–174.Google Scholar
  16. Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2002). Interrelations among social-cognitive skills in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(2), 91–106.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Carter, A. S., Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. S., Wang, J.-J., Lord, C., Dawson, G., ... Schopler, E. (1998). The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales: Supplementary norms for individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(4), 287–302.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Čeponienė, R., Lepistö, T., Shestakova, A., Vanhala, R., Alku, P., Näätänen, R., & Yaguchi, K. (2003). Speech–sound-selective auditory impairment in children with autism: They can perceive but do not attend. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(9), 5567–5572. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0835631100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Drew, A., & Cox, A. (2003). Predicting language outcome in infants with autism and pervasive developmental disorder. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 38(3), 265–285. doi: 10.1080/136820310000104830XDYKN6D5P3YP2M6H.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Chawarska, K., Volkmar, F., & Klin, A. (2010). Limited attentional bias for faces in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(2), 178–185.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Chawarska, K., Ye, S., Shic, F., & Chen, L. (2016). Multilevel differences in spontaneous social attention in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Child Development, 87(2), 543–557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(6), 479–485. doi: 10.1023/a:1026043926488.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A. & Liaw, J. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 271–283. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.2712004-11032-012.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Gliga, T., Elsabbagh, M., Hudry, K., Charman, T., Johnson, M. H., & The BASIS Team. (2012). Gaze following, gaze reading, and word learning in children at risk for autism. Child Development, 83, 926–938. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01750.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Gotham, K., Risi, S., Pickles, A., & Lord, C. (2007). The autism diagnostic observation schedule: Revised algorithms for improved diagnostic validity. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 613–627.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Grassmann, S., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Young children follow pointing over words in interpreting acts of reference. Developmental Science, 13(1), 252–263. CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Hollich, G. J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Brand, R. J., Brown, E., Chung, H. L., ... Bloom, L. (2000). Breaking the language barrier: An emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, i–135.Google Scholar
  28. Hunnius, S. (2007). The early development of visual attention and its implications for social and cognitive development. In C. VonHofsten & K. Rosander (Eds.), From action to cognition (Vol. 164, pp. 187–209). Oxford: Elseiver.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Jaswal, V. K., & Hansen, M. B. (2006). Learning words: Children disregard some pragmatic information that conflicts with mutual exclusivity. Developmental Science, 9(2), 158–165.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Jones, W., Carr, K., & Klin, A. (2008). Absence of preferential looking to the eyes of approaching adults predicts level of social disability in 2-year-old toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(8), 946–954. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.8.946.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2013). Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2-6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature, 504(7480), 427–431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(9), 809–816. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.809.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Koegel, R. L., Shirotova, L., & Koegel, L. K. (2009). Brief report: Using individualized orienting cues to facilitate first-word acquisition in non-responders with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(11), 1587–1592.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  34. Leekam, S. R., Hunnisett, E., & Moore, C. (1998). Targets and cues: Gaze-following in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 39(7), 951–962.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Lewkowicz, D. J., & Hansen-Tift, A. M. (2012). Infants deploy selective attention to the mouth of a talking face when learning speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 1431–1436. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1114783109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., ... Rutter, M (2000). The Autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule-2nd edition (ADOS-2). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  38. Luyster, R., Lopez, K., & Lord, C. (2007). Characterizing communicative development in children referred for autism spectrum disorders using the MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventory (CDI). Journal of Child Language, 34(03), 623–654. doi: 10.1017/S0305000907008094.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Luyster, R., & Lord, C. (2009). Word learning in children with autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Psychology, 45(6), 1774.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  40. Merin, N., Young, G., Ozonoff, S., & Rogers, S. (2007). Visual fixation patterns during reciprocal social interaction distinguish a subgroup of 6-month-old infants at-risk for autism from comparison infants. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(1), 108–121.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Morales, M., Mundy, P., & Rojas, J. (1998). Following the direction of gaze and language development in 6-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 373–377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Morales, M., Mundy, P., Delgado, C. E. F., Yale, M., Messinger, D., Neal, R., & Schwartz, H. K. (2000). Responding to joint attention Across the 6- through 24-month age period and early language acquisition. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21, 283–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Mundy, P., Block, J., Delgado, C., Pomares, Y., Van Hecke, A. V., & Parlade, M. V. (2007). Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. Child Development, 78(3), 938–954. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01042.x.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  44. Nakano, T., Tanaka, K., Endo, Y., Yamane, Y., Yamamoto, T., Nakano, Y., ... Kitazawa, S. (2010). Atypical gaze patterns in children and adults with autism spectrum disorders dissociated from developmental changes in gaze behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, rspb20100587.Google Scholar
  45. Norbury, C. F., Brock, J., Cragg, L., Einav, S., Griffiths, H., & Nation, K. (2009). Eye-movement patterns are associated with communicative competence in autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(7), 834–842. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02073.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Norbury, C. F., Griffiths, H., & Nation, K. (2010). Sound before meaning: word learning in autistic disorders. Neuropsychologia, 48(14), 4012–4019.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Parish-Morris, J., Hennon, E. A., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2007). Children with autism illuminate the role of social intention in word learning. Child development, 78, 1265–1287.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Preissler, M. A., & Carey, S. (2005). The role of inferences about referential intent in word learning: evidence from autism. Cognition, 97(1), B13–B23.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Rapin, I., & Dunn, M. (2003). Update on the language disorders of individuals on the autistic spectrum. Brain and Development, 25(3), 166–172.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Rice, K., Moriuchi, J. M., Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2012). Parsing heterogeneity in autism spectrum disorders: Visual scanning of dynamic social scenes in school-aged children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(3), 238–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Robins, D., Fein, D., Barton, M., & Green, J. (2001). The Modified checklist for autism in toddlers: An initial study investigating the early detection of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(2), 131–144. doi: 10.1023/a:1010738829569.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Rutherford, M. D., Walsh, J. A., & Lee, V. (2015). Brief report: Infants developing with ASD show a unique developmental pattern of facial feature scanning. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(8), 2618–2623 (2015). doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2396-7.
  53. Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). SCQ: Social Communication Questionnaire. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
  54. Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2008). Gaze following in human infants depends on communicative signals. Current Biology, 18(9), 668–671. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.03.059.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Smith, K., Smith, A. D. M., & Blythe, R. A. (2011). Cross-situational learning: An experimental study of word-Learning mechanisms. Cognitive Science, 35(3), 480–498. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01158.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Smith, L. B. (2000). Learning how to learn words: An associative crane. Becoming a Word Learner: A Debate on Lexical Acquisition, 51–80.Google Scholar
  57. Smith, L. B., & Yu, C. (2008). Infants rapidly learn word-referent mappings via cross-situational statistics. Cognition, 106(3), 1558–1568. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.010.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Tager-Flusberg, H. (2006). Defining language phenotypes in autism. Clinical Neuroscience Research, 6(3), 219–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Tager-Flusberg, H., Paul, R., & Lord, C. (2005). Language and communication in autism. Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders, 1, 335–364.Google Scholar
  60. Tenenbaum, E., Amso, D., Abar, B., & Sheinkopf, S. J. (2014a). Attention and word learning in autistic, language delayed and typically developing children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 490.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  61. Tenenbaum, E., Shah, R. J., Sobel, D. M., Malle, B. F., & Morgan, J. L. (2013). Increased Focus on the mouth among infants in the first year of life: A longitudinal eye-tracking study. Infancy, 18(4), 534–553. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00135.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Tenenbaum, E., Sobel, D. M., Sheinkopf, S. J., Malle, B. F., & Morgan, J. L. (2014b). Attention to the mouth and gaze following in infancy predict language development. Journal of Child Language, 1–18.Google Scholar
  63. Trueswell, J. C., Medina, T. N., Hafri, A., & Gleitman, L. R. (2013). Propose but verify: Fast mapping meets cross-situational word learning. Cognitive Psychology, 66(1), 126–156.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. von Hofsten, C., Uhlig, H., Adell, M., & Kochukhova, O. (2009). How children with autism look at events. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3(2), 556–569. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2008.12.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Wechsler, D. (2002). Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence third edition (WPPSI-III). San Antonio: Pearson.Google Scholar
  66. Wetherby, A. M., Allen, L., Cleary, J., Kublin, K., & Goldstein, H. (2002). Validity and reliability of the communication and symbolic behavior scales developmental profile With very young children. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 45(6), 1202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Young, G., Merin, S., Rogers, N., S, J., &amp, Ozonoff, S. (2009). Gaze behavior and affect at 6 months: Predicting clinical outcomes and language development in typically developing infants and infants at risk for autism. Developmental Science, 12(5), 798–814.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  68. Yurovsky, D., & Frank, M. C. (2015). Beyond naïve cue combination: Salience and social cues in early word learning. Developmental Science. doi: 10.1111/desc.12349.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (2011). Preschool language scales fifth edition (PLS-5). Bloomington, MN: Pearson.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Brown Center for the Study of Children at RiskWomen and Infants HospitalProvidenceUSA
  2. 2.Department of Cognitive, Linguistic and Psychological SciencesBrown UniversityProvidenceUSA
  3. 3.Emma Pendleton Bradley HospitalEast ProvidenceUSA
  4. 4.Department of Psychiatry & Human BehaviorBrown UniversityProvidenceUSA

Personalised recommendations