American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th edn.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Book
Google Scholar
Amso, D., Haas, S., Tennebaum, E., Markant, J., & Sheinkopf, S. (2014). Bottom-up attention orienting in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 664–673.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Arias-Trejo, N., & Plunkett, K. (2009). Lexical-semantic priming effects during infancy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 3633–3647.
Article
Google Scholar
Arias-Trejo, N., & Plunkett, K. (2010). The effects of perceptual similarity and category membership on early word-referent identification. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 63–80.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Barr, D. J. (2008). Analyzing “visual world” eyetracking data using multilevel logistic regression. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 457–474.
Article
Google Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1–7. Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.
Bavin, E., Kidd, E., Prendergast, L., Baker, E., Dissanayake, C., & Prior, M. (2014). Severity of autism is related to children’s language processing. Autism Research, 7, 687–694.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bayley, N. (2006). Bayley scales of infant and toddler development (3rd edn.). San Antonio, TX: PsychCorp.
Google Scholar
Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D. (2012). At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 3253–3258.
Article
Google Scholar
Booth, R., & Happé, F. (2010). “Hunting with a knife and…fork”: Examining central coherence in autism, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and typical development with a linguistic task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 377–393.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Booth, R., & Happé, F. (2016). Evidence of reduced global processing in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. doi:10.1007/s10803-016-2724-6.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Brock, J., Norbury, C., Einav, S., & Nation, K. (2008). Do individuals with autism process words in context? Evidence from language-mediated eye-movements. Cognition, 108, 896–904.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Charman, T., Drew, A., Baird, C., & Baird, G. (2003). Measuring early language development in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder using the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (Infant Form). Journal of Child Language, 30, 213–236.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Eberhardt, M., & Nadig, A. (2016). Reduced sensitivity to context in language comprehension: A characteristic of autism spectrum disorders or of poor structural language ability? Research in Developmental Disabilities. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2016.01.017.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ellis Weismer, S., Lord, C., & Esler, A. (2010). Early language patterns of toddlers on the autism spectrum compared to toddlers with developmental delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 1259–1273.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Dale, P., Reznick, J.S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories: Users guide and technical manual, (2 nd edn.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing.
Google Scholar
Fernald, A., & Marchman, V. (2012). Individual differences in lexical processing at 18 months predict vocabulary growth in typically developing and late-talking toddlers. Child Development, 83, 203–222.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fernald, A., Perfors, A., & Marchman, V. (2006). Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 98–116.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Fernald, A., Pinto, J. P., Swingley, D., Weinberg, A., & McRoberts, G. (1998). Rapid gains in speed of verbal processing by infants in the 2nd year. Psychological Science, 9, 228–231.
Article
Google Scholar
Fernald, A., Swingley, D., & Pinto, J. P. (2001). When half a word is enough: Infants can recognize spoken words using partial phonetic information. Child Development, 72, 1003–1015.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fernald, A., Zangl, R., Portillo, A. L., & Marchman, V. (2008). Looking while listening: Using eye movements to monitor spoken language comprehension by infants and young children. In I. A. Sekerina, E. M. Fernandez & H. Clahsen (Eds.), Developmental psycholinguistics: On-line methods in children’s language processing (pp. 97–135). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
Google Scholar
Hadad, B. S., & Ziv, Y. (2015). Strong bias towards analytic perception in ASD does not necessarily come at the price of impaired integration skills. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 1499–1512.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hahn, N., Snedeker, J., & Rabagilati, H. (2015). Rapid ambiguity resolution in young children with autism spectrum disorder: Eye tracking evidence for the limits of weak central coherence. Autism Research, 8, 717–726.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hala, S., Pexman, P., & Glenwright, M. (2007). Priming the meaning of homographs in typically developing children and children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 329–340.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Happé, F., & Booth, R. (2008). The power of the positive: Revisiting weak coherence in autism spectrum disorders. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 50–63.
Article
Google Scholar
Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak central coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Henderson, L., Clarke, P., & Snowling, M. (2011). Accessing and selecting word meaning in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 964–973.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Henderson, L., Powell, A., Gaskell, M. G., & Norbury, C. (2014). Learning and consolidation of new spoken words in autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Science, 17, 858–871.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Houston-Price, C., Mather, E., & Sakkalou, E. (2007). Discrepancy between parental report of infants’ receptive vocabulary and infants’ behaviour in a preferential looking task. Journal of Child Language, 34, 701–724.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hudry, K., Ledbitter, K., Temple, K., Slonims, V., McConahie, H., Aldred, C., Howlin, P., Charman, T., & The PACT Consortium. (2010). Preschools with autism show greater impairment in receptive compared with expressive language abilities. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 45, 681–690.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Huettig, F., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2005). Word meaning and the control of eye fixation: Semantic competitor effects and the visual world paradigm. Cognition, 96, B23–B32.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Järvinen-Pasley, A., Wallace, G. L., Ramus, F., Happé, F., & Heaton, P. (2008). Enhanced perceptual processing of speech in autism. Developmental Science, 11, 109–121.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kaldy, Z., Kraper, C., Carter, A. S., & Blaser, E. (2011). Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorder are more successful at visual search than typically developing toddlers. Developmental Science, 14, 980–988.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Kamio, Y., Robins, D., Kelley, E., Swainson, B., & Fein, D. (2007). Atypical lexical/semantic processing in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders without early language delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1116–1122.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kamio, Y., & Toichi, M. (2000). Dual access to semantics in autism: Is pictorial access superior to verbal access? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 859–867.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
López, B., & Leekam, S. (2003). Do children with autism fail to process information in context? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 285–300.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Lord, C., Luyster, R., Gotham, K., & Guthrie, W. (2012). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition (ADOS-2) Manual (Part 2): Toddler Module. Torrence, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Google Scholar
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition (ADOS-2) Manual (Part 1): Modules 1–4. Torrence, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Google Scholar
Mani, N., & Plunkett, K. (2010). In the infant’s mind’s ear: Evidence for implicit naming in 18-month-olds. Psychological Science, 21, 908–913.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Mani, N., & Plunkett, K. (2011). Phonological priming and cohort effects in toddlers. Cognition, 121, 196–206.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Mahr, T., Macmillan, B., Saffran, J., & Ellis Weismer, S., & Edwards, J. (2015). Anticipatory coarticulation facilitates word recognition in toddlers. Cognition, 142, 345–350.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A., Adams, K., Loi, E., Fernald, A., & Feldman, H. M. (2016). Early language processing efficiency predicts later receptive vocabulary outcomes in children born preterm. Child Neuropsychology, 17, 649–665.
Article
Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A. & Fernald, A. (2008). Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science, 11, F9–F16.
Marslen-Wilson, W., & Zwitserlood, P. (1989). Accessing spoken words: The importance of word onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 576–585.
Google Scholar
McMurray, B., Samelson, V. M., Lee, S. H., & Tomblin, J. B. (2010). Individual differences in online spoken word recognition: Implications for SLI. Cognitive Psychology, 60, 1–39.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Mirman, D. (2014). Growth Curve Analysis and Visualization Using R. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Google Scholar
Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 27–43.
Norbury, C. F. (2005). Barking up the wrong tree? Lexical ambiguity resolution in children with language impairments and autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90, 142–171.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Norbury, C. F., & Bishop, D.V.M. (2002). Inferential processing and story recall in children with communication problems: A comparison of specific language impairment, pragmatic language impairment and high-functioning autism. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 37, 227–251.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
O’Riordan, M. A. (2004). Superior visual search in adults with autism. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 8, 229–248.
Article
Google Scholar
Pellicano, E. (2011). Psychological models of autism: An overview. In I. Roth & P. Rezaie (Eds.), Researching the autism spectrum: Contemporary perspectives (pp. 219–265). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced visual search for a conjunctive target in autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 777–783.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ray-Subramanian, C., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2012). Receptive and expressive language as predictors of restricted and repetitive behaviors in young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism Developmental Disorders, 42, 2113–2120.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Riches, N., Loucas, T., Baird, G., Charman, T., & Simonoff, E. (2016). Elephants in pyjamas: Testing the weak central coherence account of autism spectrum disorders using a syntactic disambiguation task. Journal of Autism Developmental Disorders, 46, 155–163.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Robins, D., Fein, D., & Barton, M. (1999). Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT™). Storrs, CT: Self-published.
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social communication questionnaire. Los Angeles, CA: Western psychological Services.
Google Scholar
Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Autism diagnostic interview-revised. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Google Scholar
Smith, L., & Yu, C. (2008). Infants rapidly learn word-referent mappings via cross-situational statistics. Cognition, 106, 1558–1568.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Styles, S., & Plunkett, K. (2009). How do infants build a semantic system? Language and Cognition, 1, 1–24.
Article
Google Scholar
Swingley, D., Pinto, J., & Fernald, A. (1999). Continuous processing in word recognition at 24 months. Cognition, 71, 73–108.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Venker, C. E., Eernisse, E. R., Saffran, J. R., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2013). Individual differences in the real-time comprehension of children with ASD. Autism Research, 6, 417–432.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Volden, J., Smith, I. M., Szatmari, P., Bryson, S., Fombonne, E., Mirenda, P., et al. (2011). Using the Preschool Language Scale, fourth edition to characterize language in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 20, 200–208.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
White, S., & Saldaña, D. (2011). Performance of children with autism on the embedded figures test: A closer look at a popular task. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 1565–1572.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar