Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 48, Issue 4, pp 1382–1396 | Cite as

Seeing the Forest and the Trees: Default Local Processing in Individuals with High Autistic Traits Does Not Come at the Expense of Global Attention

  • Ryan A. Stevenson
  • Sol Z. Sun
  • Naomi Hazlett
  • Jonathan S. Cant
  • Morgan D. Barense
  • Susanne Ferber
S.I. : Local vs. Global processing in Autism Spectrum Disorders

Abstract

Atypical sensory perception is one of the most ubiquitous symptoms of autism, including a tendency towards a local-processing bias. We investigated whether local-processing biases were associated with global-processing impairments on a global/local attentional-scope paradigm in conjunction with a composite-face task. Behavioural results were related to individuals’ levels of autistic traits, specifically the Attention to Detail subscale of the Autism Quotient, and the Sensory Profile Questionnaire. Individuals showing high rates of Attention to Detail were more susceptible to global attentional-scope manipulations, suggesting that local-processing biases associated with Attention to Detail do not come at the cost of a global-processing deficit, but reflect a difference in default global versus local bias. This relationship operated at the attentional/perceptual level, but not response criterion.

Keywords

Autism spectrum disorder Face recognition Sensory processing Composite-face effect Vision Attention Global processing Local processing 

Notes

Author contribution

R.S.—NSERC Banting Postdoctoral Fellowship, CIHR Autism Research Training program; S.S.—NSERC CGS-M Scholarship; J.C.—NSERC Discovery Grant 435647-2013; M.B.—Scholar Award from the James S McDonnell Foundation, CIHR Grant MOP-115148; S.F.—NSERC Grant 216203-13, CIHR Grant 106436.

References

  1. Almeida, R. A., Dickinson, J. E., Maybery, M. T., Badcock, J. C., & Badcock, D. R. (2014). Enhanced global integration of closed contours in individuals with high levels of autistic-like traits. Vision research, 103, 109–115.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. American Psychiatric Association, American Psychiatric Association, DSM-5 Task Force. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5, 5th edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  3. Association, A. P. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, text revision (DSM-IV-TR). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  4. Badcock, J. C., Whitworth, F. A., Badcock, D. R., & Lovegrove, W. J. (1990). Low-frequency filtering and the processing of local-global stimuli. Perception, 19, 617–629.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Jolliffe, Therese. (1997). Is there a” language of the eyes”? Evidence from normal adults, and adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Visual Cognition, 4, 311–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001a). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, malesand females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001b). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 241–251.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Baum, S. H., Stevenson, R. A., & Wallace, M. T. (2015). Behavioral, perceptual, and neural alterations in sensory and multisensory function in autism spectrum disorder. Progress in Neurobiology, 134, 140–160.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  9. Behrmann, M., et al. (2006a). Configural processing in autism and its relationship to face processing. Neuropsychologia, 44, 110–129.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Behrmann, M., Thomas, C., & Humphreys, K. (2006b). Seeing it differently: Visual processing in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 258–264.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2005). Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity. Brain, 128, 2430–2441. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh561.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Blair, R., Frith, U., Smith, N., Abell, F., & Cipolotti, L. (2002). Fractionation of visual memory: Agency detection and its impairment in autism. Neuropsychologia, 40, 108–118.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Bölte, S., Holtmann, M., Poustka, F., Scheurich, A., & Schmidt, L. (2007). Gestalt perception and local-global processing in high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1493–1504.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face recognition in relatively able autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 843–859.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Brown, C., & Dunn, W. (2002). Adolescent-adult sensory profile: User’s manual. Therapy Skill Builders.Google Scholar
  16. Brown, C., Tollefson, N., Dunn, W., Cromwell, R., & Filion, D. (2001). The adult sensory profile: Measuring patterns of sensory processing. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 55, 75–82.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Bruinsma, Y., Koegel, R. L., & Koegel, L. K. (2004). Joint attention and children with autism: A review of the literature. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 10, 169–175.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Busigny, T., & Rossion, B. (2011). Holistic processing impairment can be restricted to faces in acquired prosopagnosia: Evidence from the global/local Navon effect. Journal of Neuropsychology, 5, 1–14.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Cheung, O. S., Richler, J. J., Palmeri, T. J., & Gauthier, I. (2008). Revisiting the role of spatial frequencies in the holistic processing of faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1327.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. De Martino, B., Harrison, N. A., Knafo, S., Bird, G., & Dolan, R. J. (2008). Explaining enhanced logical consistency during decision making in autism. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 10746–10750.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Deruelle, C., Rondan, C., Gepner, B., & Fagot, J. (2006). Processing of compound visual stimuli by children with autism and Asperger syndrome. International Journal of Psychology, 41, 97–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Deruelle, C., Rondan, C., Gepner, B., & Tardif, C. (2004). Spatial frequency and face processing in children with autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 199–210.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Dunn, W., & Westman, K. (1997). The sensory profile: The performance of a national sample of children without disabilities. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 51, 25–34.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural differences in facial expression of emotion. In: J. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1971. (pp. 207–283). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  25. Eriksen, C. W., & James, J. D. S. (1986). Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model. Perception and Psychophysics, 40, 225–240.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Foss-Feig, J. H., et al. (2010). An extended multisensory temporal binding window in autism spectrum disorders. Experimental Brain Research, 203, 381–389. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2240-4.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  27. Foxe, J. J., et al. (2013). Severe multisensory speech integration deficits in high-functioning school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and their resolution during early adolescence. Cerebral Cortex, bht213.Google Scholar
  28. Frith, U., & Happe, F. (1994). Autism: beyond “theory of mind”. Cognition, 50, 115–132.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Gao, Z., Flevaris, A. V., Robertson, L. C., & Bentin, S. (2011). Priming global and local processing of composite faces: revisiting the processing-bias effect on face perception. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 1477–1486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Gauthier, I., Klaiman, C., & Schultz, R. T. (2009). Face composite effects reveal abnormal face processing in Autism spectrum disorders. Vision Research, 49, 470–478. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.12.007.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. Gepner, B., Gelder, B. D., & Schonen, S. D. (1996). Face processing in autistics: Evidence for a generalised deficit? Child Neuropsychology, 2, 123–139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Goffaux, V., & Rossion, B. (2006). Faces are” spatial”—holistic face perception is supported by low spatial frequencies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 1023.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Gomot, M., et al. (2006). Change detection in children with autism: an auditory event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage, 29, 475–484.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Gomot, M., Giard, M.-H., Adrien, J.-L., Barthelemy, C., & Bruneau, N. (2002). Hypersensitivity to acoustic change in children with autism: Electrophysiological evidence of left frontal cortex dysfunctioning. Psychophysiology, 39, 577–584.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Happé, F. G. (1996). Studying weak central coherence at low levels: Children with autism do not succumb to visual illusions. A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 873–877.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Harms, M. B., Martin, A., & Wallace, G. L. (2010). Facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: A review of behavioral and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychology Review, 20, 290–322.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Hauck, M., Fein, D., Maltby, N., Waterhouse, L., & Feinstein, C. (1998). Memory for faces in children with autism. Child Neuropsychology, 4, 187–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Hills, P. J., & Lewis, M. B. (2009). A spatial frequency account of the detriment that local processing of Navon letters has on face recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1427.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Hoekstra, R. A., Bartels, M., Cath, D. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2008). Factor structure, reliability and criterion validity of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): A study in Dutch population and patient groups. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1555–1566.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  41. Hughes, H. C., Layton, W. M., Baird, J. C., & Lester, L. S. (1984). Global precedence in visual pattern recognition. Perception and Psychophysics, 35, 361–371.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Hurst, R., Mitchell, J., Kimbrel, N., Kwapil, T., & Nelson-Gray, R. (2007). Examination of the reliability and factor structure of the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) in a non-clinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 1938–1949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Iarocci, G., Burack, J. A., Shore, D. I., Mottron, L., & Enns, J. T. (2006). Global–local visual processing in high functioning children with autism: structural vs. implicit task biases. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 117–129. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0045-2.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Iarocci, G., & McDonald, J. (2006). Sensory integration and the perceptual experience of persons with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 77–90. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0044-3.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Iarocci, G., Rombough, A., Yager, J., Weeks, D. J., & Chua, R. (2010). Visual influences on speech perception in children with autism. Autism, 14, 305–320. doi: 10.1177/1362361309353615.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Irwin, J. R., Tornatore, L. A., Brancazio, L., & Whalen, D. H. (2011). Can children with autism spectrum disorders “hear” a speaking face? Child Development, 82, 1397–1403. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01619.x.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  47. Jacques, C., & Rossion, B. (2009). The initial representation of individual faces in the right occipito-temporal cortex is holistic: Electrophysiological evidence from the composite face illusion. Journal of Vision, 9, 8.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Jacques, C., & Rossion, B. (2010). Misaligning face halves increases and delays the N170 specifically for upright faces: Implications for the nature of early face representations. Brain Research, 1318, 96–109.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Johnson, S. A., Blaha, L. M., Houpt, J. W., & Townsend, J. T. (2010). Systems factorial technology provides new insights on global–local information processing in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 54, 53–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jmp.2009.06.006.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Are people with autism and Asperger syndrome faster than normal on the Embedded Figures Test? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 527–534.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Joseph, R. M., Keehn, B., Connolly, C., Wolfe, J. M., & Horowitz, T. S. (2009). Why is visual search superior in autism spectrum disorder? Developmental science, 12, 1083–1096.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.Google Scholar
  53. Kemner, C., Van Ewijk, L., Van Engeland, H., & Hooge, I. (2008). Brief report: Eye movements during visual search tasks indicate enhanced stimulus discriminability in subjects with PDD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 553–557.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Kenny, L., Hattersley, C., Molins, B., Buckley, C., Povey, C., & Pellicano, E. (2015). Which terms should be used to describe autism? Perspectives from the UK autism community. Autism. doi: 10.1177/1362361315588200.
  55. Klin, A., Sparrow, S. S., de Bildt, A., Cicchetti, D. V., Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. R. (1999). A normed study of face recognition in autism and related disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 499–508.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Koldewyn, K., Jiang, Y. V., Weigelt, S., & Kanwisher, N. (2013). Global/local processing in autism: Not a disability, but a disinclination. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 2329–2340.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  57. Kuefner, D., Jacques, C., Prieto, E. A., & Rossion, B. (2010). Electrophysiological correlates of the composite face illusion: Disentangling perceptual and decisional components of holistic face processing in the human brain. Brain and Cognition, 74, 225–238.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Kwakye, L. D., Foss-Feig, J. H., Cascio, C. J., Stone, W. L., & Wallace, M. T. (2011). Altered auditory and multisensory temporal processing in autism spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 4, 129. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2010.00129.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  59. Lowe, M. X., et al. (2015). Sensory processing patterns predict the integration of information held in visual working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000131.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Maurer, D., Le Grand, R., & Mondloch, C. J. (2002). The many faces of configural processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 255–260.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., & Siegel, D. J. (1997). Neuropsychologic functioning in autism: Profile of a complex information processing disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 303–316.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Mottron, L., Burack, J. A., Iarocci, G., Belleville, S., & Enns, J. T. (2003). Locally oriented perception with intact global processing among adolescents with high-functioning autism: evidence from multiple paradigms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 904–913.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulieres, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: an update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 27–43. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0040-7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Müller, N. G., Bartelt, O. A., Donner, T. H., Villringer, A., & Brandt, S. A. (2003). A physiological correlate of the “zoom lens” of visual attention. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 3561–3565.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Navon, D. (1981). The forest revisited: More on global precedence. Psychological Research, 43, 1–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Nishimura, M., Rutherford, M., & Maurer, D. (2008). Converging evidence of configural processing of faces in high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorders. Visual Cognition, 16, 859–891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. O’Riordan, M., & Plaisted, K. (2001). Enhanced discrimination in autism. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 961–979.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. O’Riordan, M. A., Plaisted, K. C., Driver, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Superior visual search in autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 719.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. O’riordan, M. A. (2004). Superior visual search in adults with autism. Autism, 8, 229–248.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Pellicano, E., & Burr, D. (2012). When the world becomes ‘too real’: A Bayesian explanation of autistic perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 504–510.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Pellicano, E., Gibson, L., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., & Badcock, D. R. (2005). Abnormal global processing along the dorsal visual pathway in autism: A possible mechanism for weak visuospatial coherence? Neuropsychologia, 43, 1044–1053.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Plaisted, K., Swettenham, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Children with autism show local precedence in a divided attention task and global precedence in a selective attention task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 733–742.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Rauschenberger, R., & Yantis, S. (2001). Attentional capture by globally defined objects. Perception and Psychophysics, 63, 1250–1261.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Richler, J. J., Tanaka, J. W., Brown, D. D., & Gauthier, I. (2008). Why does selective attention to parts fail in face processing? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 1356.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Moss, S. A., Brereton, A. V., & Tonge, B. J. (2000). Atypical interference of local detail on global processing in high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 769–778.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Rossion, B. (2013). The composite face illusion: A whole window into our understanding of holistic face perception. Visual Cognition, 21, 139–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Scaife, M., & Bruner, J. S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature, 253, 265–266.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Schiltz, C., Dricot, L., Goebel, R., & Rossion, B. (2010). Holistic perception of individual faces in the right middle fusiform gyrus as evidenced by the composite face illusion. Journal of Vision, 10, 25.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24, 613–620.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design task? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 1351–1364.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Smith, E. G., & Bennetto, L. (2007). Audiovisual speech integration and lipreading in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 813–821. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01766.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  83. Stevenson, R. A., Segers, M., Ferber, S., Barense, M. D., Camarata, S., & Wallace, M. T. (2015). Keeping time in the brain: Autism spectrum disorder and audiovisual temporal processing. Autism Research. doi: 10.1002/aur.1566.
  84. Stevenson, R. A., Segers, M., Ferber, S., Barense, M. D., & Wallace, M. T. (2014a). The impact of multisensory integration deficits on speech perception in children with autism spectrum disorders. Front Psychol, 5, 379. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00379.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  85. Stevenson, R. A., et al. (2014b). Multisensory temporal integration in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 691–697. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3615-13.2014.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  86. Stevenson, R. A., et al. (2014c). Brief report: Arrested development of audiovisual speech perception in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 1470–1477. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1992-7.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  87. Stevenson, R. A., et al. (2014d). Evidence for diminished multisensory integration in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 3161–3167. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2179-6.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  88. Stewart, M. E., & Austin, E. J. (2009). The structure of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from a student sample in Scotland. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 224–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Sumby, W. H., & Pollack, I. (1954). Visual contribution to speech intelligibility in noise. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 26, 212–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Tanaka, J. W., & Farah, M. J. (1993). Parts and wholes in face recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 225–245.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  91. Tanaka, J. W., & Sung, A. (2013). The “eye avoidance” hypothesis of autism face processing. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1976-7.Google Scholar
  92. Tantam, D., Monaghan, L., Nicholson, H., & Stirling, J. (1989). Autistic children’s ability to interpret faces: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 623–630.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  93. Teunisse, J.-P., & de Gelder, B. (2003). Face processing in adolescents with autistic disorder: The inversion and composite effects. Brain and Cognition, 52, 285–294.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  94. Van der Hallen, R., Evers, K., Brewaeys, K., Van den Noortgate, W., & Wagemans, J. (2014). Global processing takes time: A meta-analysis on local-global visual processing in ASD. Psychological bulletin, 141, 549–573. doi: 10.1037/bul0000004.
  95. Verde, M. F., Macmillan, N. A., & Rotello, C. M. (2006). Measures of sensitivity based on a single hit rate and false alarm rate: The accuracy, precision, and robustness of′, A z, and A’. Perception and Psychophysics, 68, 643–654.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  96. Wallace, S., Coleman, M., & Bailey, A. (2008). An investigation of basic facial expression recognition in autism spectrum disorders. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 1353–1380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Wallace, M. T., & Stevenson, R. A. (2014). The construct of the multisensory temporal binding window and its dysregulation in developmental disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 64, 105–123.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  98. Wang, A. T., Dapretto, M., Hariri, A. R., Sigman, M., & Bookheimer, S. Y. (2004). Neural correlates of facial affect processing in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 481–490.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  99. Williams, J. H., Massaro, D. W., Peel, N. J., Bosseler, A., & Suddendorf, T. (2004). Visual-auditory integration during speech imitation in autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 25, 559–575. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2004.01.008.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  100. Woynaroski, T. G., Kwakye, L. D., Foss-Feig, J. H., Stevenson, R. A., Stone, W. L., & Wallace, M. T. (2013). Multisensory speech perception in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1836-5.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  101. Young, A. W., Hellawell, D., & Hay, D. C. (1987). Configurational information in face perception. Perception, 16, 747–759.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ryan A. Stevenson
    • 1
  • Sol Z. Sun
    • 1
    • 2
  • Naomi Hazlett
    • 1
  • Jonathan S. Cant
    • 2
  • Morgan D. Barense
    • 1
    • 3
  • Susanne Ferber
    • 1
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of TorontoTorontoCanada
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of Toronto ScarboroughTorontoCanada
  3. 3.Rotman Research Institute at BaycrestTorontoCanada

Personalised recommendations