Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 46, Issue 1, pp 305–314 | Cite as

Brief Report: Patterns of Eye Movements in Face to Face Conversation are Associated with Autistic Traits: Evidence from a Student Sample

Brief Report

Abstract

The current study investigated whether the amount of autistic traits shown by an individual is associated with viewing behaviour during a face-to-face interaction. The eye movements of 36 neurotypical university students were recorded using a mobile eye-tracking device. High amounts of autistic traits were neither associated with reduced looking to the social partner overall, nor with reduced looking to the face. However, individuals who were high in autistic traits exhibited reduced visual exploration during the face-to-face interaction overall, as demonstrated by shorter and less frequent saccades. Visual exploration was not related to social anxiety. This study suggests that there are systematic individual differences in visual exploration during social interactions and these are related to amount of autistic traits.

Keywords

Social attention Mobile eye-tracking Eye movements Autistic traits 

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
  2. Argyle, M. (1988). Bodily communication (2nd ed., p. 384). London: Methuen.Google Scholar
  3. Barnes, K. A., Howard, J. H, Jr, Howard, D. V., Gilotty, L., Kenworthy, L., Gaillard, W. D., et al. (2008). Intact implicit learning of spatial context and temporal sequences in childhood autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychology, 22(5), 563–570.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001a). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 241–251.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001b). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2005). Gaze and arrow cueing of attention reveals individual differences along the autism spectrum as a function of target context. British Journal of Psychology, 96(1), 95–114.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2006). predictive gaze cues and personality judgments should eye trust you? Psychological Science, 17(6), 514–520.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brown, J., Aczel, B., Jimenez, L., Kaufman, S. B., & Grant, K. P. (2010). Intact implicit learning in autism spectrum conditions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(9), 1789–1812.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Burgoon, J. K., Manusov, V., Mineo, P., & Hale, J. L. (1985). Effects of gaze on hiring, credibility, attraction and relational message interpretation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 9(3), 133–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Chen, F. S., & Yoon, J. M. D. (2011). Brief report: Broader autism phenotype predicts spontaneous reciprocity of direct gaze. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(8), 1131–1134.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E. S., & Schultz, R. T. (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 231–239.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Chevallier, C., Parish-Morris, J., McVey, A., Rump, K. M., Sasson, N. J., Herrington, J. D., & Schultz, R. T. (2015). Measuring social attention and motivation in autism spectrum disorder using eye-tracking: Stimulus type matters. Autism Research,. doi:10.1002/aur.1479.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Constantino, J. N., & Gruber, C. P. (2002). The social responsiveness scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
  14. Corden, B., Chilvers, R., & Skuse, D. (2008). Avoidance of emotionally arousing stimuli predicts social–perceptual impairment in Asperger’s syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 46(1), 137–147.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(6), 479–485.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Doherty-Sneddon, G., Bruce, V., Bonner, L., Longbotham, S., & Doyle, C. (2002). Development of gaze aversion as disengagement from visual information. Developmental Psychology, 38(3), 438.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Doherty-Sneddon, G., & Phelps, F. G. (2005). Gaze aversion: A response to cognitive or social difficulty? Memory & Cognition, 33(4), 727–733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Elison, J. T., Sasson, N. J., Turner-Brown, L. M., Dichter, G. S., & Bodfish, J. W. (2012). Age trends in visual exploration of social and nonsocial information in children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(2), 842–851.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Fletcher-Watson, S., Leekam, S. R., Benson, V., Frank, M. C., & Findlay, J. M. (2009). Eye movements reveal attention to social information in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 47(1), 248–257.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Freeth, M., Bullock, T., & Milne, E. (2013a). The distribution of and relationship between autistic traits and social anxiety in a UK student population. Autism, 17(5), 571–581.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Freeth, M., Chapman, P., Ropar, D., & Mitchell, P. (2010). Do gaze cues in complex scenes capture and direct the attention of high functioning adolescents with ASD? Evidence from eye-tracking. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(5), 534–547.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Freeth, M., Foulsham, T., & Kingstone, A. (2013b). What affects social attention? Social presence, eye contact and autistic traits. PLoS One, 8(1), e53286.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Glenberg, A. M., Schroeder, J. L., & Robertson, D. A. (1998). Averting the gaze disengages the environment and facilitates remembering. Memory & Cognition, 26(4), 651–658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Goldberg, M. C., Lasker, A. G., Zee, D. S., Garth, E., Tien, A., & Landa, R. J. (2002). Deficits in the initiation of eye movements in the absence of a visual target in adolescents with high functioning autism. Neuropsychologia, 40(12), 2039–2049.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Guillon, Q., Hadjikhani, N., Baduel, S., & Rogé, B. (2014). Visual social attention in autism spectrum disorder: Insights from eye tracking studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 42, 279–297.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hanley, M., McPhillips, M., Mulhern, G., & Riby, D. M. (2013). Spontaneous attention to faces in Asperger syndrome using ecologically valid static stimuli. Autism, 17(6), 754–761.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hanley, M., Riby, D. M., McCormack, T., Carty, C., Coyle, L., Crozier, N., et al. (2014). Attention during social interaction in children with autism: Comparison to specific language impairment, typical development, and links to social cognition. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8(7), 908–924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hernandez, N., Metzger, A., Magné, R., Bonnet-Brilhault, F., Roux, S., Barthelemy, C., & Martineau, J. (2009). Exploration of core features of a human face by healthy and autistic adults analyzed by visual scanning. Neuropsychologia, 47(4), 1004–1012.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hsiao, J. H., & Cottrell, G. (2008). Two fixations suffice in face recognition. Psychological Science, 19(10), 998–1006.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hurley, R. S. E., Losh, M., Parlier, M., Reznick, J. S., & Piven, J. (2007). The broad autism phenotype questionnaire. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(9), 1679–1690.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Ingersoll, B., Hopwood, C. J., Wainer, A., & Donnellan, M. B. (2011). A comparison of three self-report measures of the broader autism phenotype in a non-clinical sample. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(12), 1646–1657.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kemner, C., Verbaten, M. N., Cuperus, J. M., Camfferman, G., & Van Engeland, H. (1998). Abnormal saccadic eye movements in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(1), 61–67.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kikuchi, Y., Senju, A., Akechi, H., Tojo, Y., Osanai, H., & Hasegawa, T. (2011). Atypical disengagement from faces and its modulation by the control of eye fixation in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(5), 629–645.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(9), 809–816.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Kourkoulou, A., Kuhn, G., Findlay, J. M., & Leekam, S. R. (2013). Eye movement difficulties in autism spectrum disorder: Implications for implicit contextual learning. Autism Research, 6(3), 177–189.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Laidlaw, K. E., Foulsham, T., Kuhn, G., & Kingstone, A. (2011). Potential social interactions are important to social attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(14), 5548–5553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Landry, R., & Bryson, S. E. (2004). Impaired disengagement of attention in young children withautism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(6), 1115–1122.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Liebowitz, M. R. (1987). Social phobia. Modern Problems of Pharmacopsychiatry, 22, 141–173.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. McPartland, J. C., Webb, S. J., Keehn, B., & Dawson, G. (2011). Patterns of visual attention to faces and objects in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(2), 148–157.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Nadig, A., Lee, I., Singh, L., Bosshart, K., & Ozonoff, S. (2010). How does the topic of conversation affect verbal exchange and eye gaze? A comparison between typical development and high functioning autism. Neuropsychologia, 48(9), 2730–2739.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Nakano, T., Tanaka, K., Endo, Y., Yamane, Y., Yamamoto, T., Nakano, Y., et al. (2010). A typical gaze patterns in children and adults with autism spectrum disorders dissociated from developmental changes in gaze behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277(1696), 2935–2943.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Noris, B., Nadel, J., Barker, M., Hadjikhani, N., & Billard, A. (2012). Investigating gaze of children with ASD in naturalistic settings. PLoS One, 7(9), e44144.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. O’Riordan, M. A. (2004). Superior visual search in adults with autism. Autism, 8(3), 229–248.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. O’Riordan, M. A., Plaisted, K. C., Driver, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Superior visual search in autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 719–730.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Pelphrey, K. A., Sasson, N. J., Reznick, J. S., Paul, G., Goldman, B. D., & Piven, J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(4), 249–261.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Peterson, M. F., & Eckstein, M. P. (2012). Looking just below the eyes is optimal across face recognition tasks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(48), E3314–E3323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced visual search for a conjunctive target in autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39(5), 777–783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Riby, D. M., & Hancock, P. J. B. (2008). Viewing it differently: Social scene perception in Williams syndrome and autism. Neuropsychologia, 46(11), 2855–2860.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Riby, D. M., & Hancock, P. J. (2009). Do faces capture the attention of individuals with Williams syndrome or autism? Evidence from tracking eye movements. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(3), 421–431.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Risko, E. F., Laidlaw, K. E., Freeth, M., Foulsham, T., & Kingstone, A. (2012). Social attention with real versus reel stimuli: Toward an empirical approach to concerns about ecological validity. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 6, 143.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Sasson, N. J., & Touchstone, E. W. (2014). Visual attention to competing social and object images by preschool children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(3), 584–592.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Sasson, N. J., Turner-Brown, L. M., Holtzclaw, T. N., Lam, K. S., & Bodfish, J. W. (2008). Children with autism demonstrate circumscribed attention during passive viewing of complex social and non-social picture arrays. Autism Research, 1(1), 31–42.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Psychology DepartmentUniversity of SheffieldSheffieldUK

Personalised recommendations