Abstract
Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) aged 11–13 (N = 16) and an IQ-matched typically developing (TD) group aged 7–12 (N = 16) completed a graded grammaticality judgment task, as well as a standardized test of cognitive function. In a departure from previous studies, the judgment task involved verb argument structure overgeneralization errors (e.g., *Lisa fell the cup off the shelf) of the type sometimes observed amongst typically developing children, as well as grammatical control sentences with the same verbs (e.g., The cup fell off the shelf). The ASD group showed a smaller dispreference for ungrammatical sentences (relative to the control sentences) than did the TD group. These findings are indicative of a subtle grammatical impairment in even relatively high-functioning children with ASD.
This is a preview of subscription content,
to check access.
Notes
Incidentally, the introduction of DSM-5 has seen a decrease in the number of individuals diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder, as compared with DSM-IV-TR Autistic disorder (AD) and pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS); see Kulage et al. (2014) for a meta-analysis.
Although we also administered the child version of the Reading the mind in the eyes test revised version (Baron-Cohen et al. 2001) we did not analyse these results, as a reviewer raised concern regarding the use of this test as a diagnostic measure.
Composite scores were not calculated, as this requires the use of age-scaled scores, which is not appropriate, given that the aim is to match the two differently-aged groups on raw performance.
References
Alarcon, M., Yonan, A. L., Gilliam, T. C., Cantor, R. M., & Geschwind, D. H. (2005). Quantitative genome scan and ordered-subsets analysis of autism endophenotypes support language QTLs. Molecular Psychiatry, 10(8), 747–757.
Allen, D. A., & Rapin, I. (1980). Language disorders in preschool children: Predictors of outcome—a preliminary report. Brain and Development, 2(1), 73–80.
Ambridge, B., Pine, J. M., Rowland, C. F., Jones, R. L., & Clark, V. (2009). A semantics-based approach to the “no negative evidence” problem. Cognitive Science, 33(7), 1301–1316.
Ambridge, B., Pine, J. P., Rowland, C. F., & Young, C. R. (2008). The effect of verb semantic class and verb frequency (entrenchment) on children’s and adults’ graded judgements of argument–structure overgeneralisation errors. Cognition, 106, 87–129.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Scahill, V., Lawson, J., & Spong, A. (2001). Are intuitive physics and intuitive psychology independent? A test with children with Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Developmental and Learning Disorders, 5, 47–78.
Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2012). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes (statistical package).
Bennetto, L., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1996). Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Child Development, 67(4), 1816–1835.
Blackwell, A., & Bates, E. (1995). Inducing agrammatic profiles in normals: Evidence for the selective vulnerability of morphology under cognitive resource limitations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 228–257.
Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003). Autism, primary pragmatic difficulties, and specific language impairment: Can we distinguish them using psycholinguistic markers? Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 45, 515–524.
Boucher, J. (1976). Articulation in early childhood autism. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 6, 297–302.
Boucher, J. (1988). Word fluency in high functioning autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18, 637–645.
Bradford, Y., Haines, J., Hutcheson, H., Gardiner, M., Braun, T., Sheffield, V., et al. (2001). Incorporating language phenotypes strengthens evidence of linkage to autism. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 105(6), 539–547.
Brock, J., Norbury, C., Einav, S., & Nation, K. (2008). Do individuals with autism process words in context? Evidence from language-mediated eye-movements. Cognition, 108(3), 896–904.
Brooks, P. J., & Tomasello, M. (1999). How children constrain their argument structure constructions. Language, 75(4), 720–738.
Brooks, P. J., Tomasello, M., Dodson, K., & Lewis, L. B. (1999). Young children’s overgeneralizations with fixed transitivity verbs. Child Development, 70(6), 1325–1337.
Brooks, P. J., & Zizak, O. (2002). Does preemption help children learn verb transitivity? Journal of Child Language, 29, 759–781.
Conti-Ramsden, G., Simkin, Z., & Botting, N. (2006). The prevalence of autistic spectrum disorders in adolescents with a history of specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(6), 621–628.
Eaves, L., & Ho, H. (2004). The very early identification of autism: Outcome at age 4½–5. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 367–378.
Eigsti, I. G., & Bennetto, L. (2009). Grammaticality judgements in autism: Deviance or delay. Journal of Child Language, 36, 999–1021.
Eigsti, I. M., Bennetto, L., & Dadlani, M. (2007). Beyond pragmatics: Morpho-syntactic development in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(6), 1007–1023.
Fombonne, E., Bolton, P., Prior, J., Jordan, H., & Rutter, M. (1997). A family study of autism: Cognitive patterns and levels in parents and siblings. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(6), 667–683.
Goodwin, A., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. R. (2012). Comprehension of wh-questions precedes their production in typical development and autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 5(2), 109–123.
Happé, F. (1999). Autism: cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(6), 216–222.
Howlin, P. (1984). The acquisition of grammatical morphemes in autistic children: A critique and replication of the findings of Bartolucci, Pierce, and Streiner, 1980. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14(2), 127–136.
Jarrold, C., Boucher, J., & Russell, J. (1997). Language profiles in children with autism: Theoretical and methodological implications. Autism, 1, 57–76.
Kjelgaard, M. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16(2/3), 287–308.
Kulage, K. M., Smaldone, A. M., & Cohn, E. G. (2014). How will DSM-5 affect autism diagnosis? A systematic literature review and meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(8), 1918–1932.
Lord, C., Risi, S., & Pickles, A. (2004). Trajectory of language development in autistic spectrum disorders. In M. Rice & S. F. Warren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies (pp. 1–38). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Luyster, R., Kadlec, M., Carter, A., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2008). Language assessment and development in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1305–1320.
Luyster, R., Lopez, K., & Lord, C. (2007). Characterizing communicative development in children referred for autism spectrum disorders using the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory (CDI). Journal of Child Language, 34, 623–654.
McDaniel, D., & Cairns, H. (1996). Eliciting judgements of grammaticality and reference. In D. McDaniel, C. McKee, & H. Cairns (Eds.), Methods for assessing children’s syntax (pp. 233–255). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Naigles, L. R., Kelty, E., Jaffery, R., & Fein, D. (2011). Abstractness and continuity in the syntactic development of young children with autism. Autism Research, 4(6), 422–437.
Naigles, L. R., & Tovar, A. T. (2012). Portable intermodal preferential looking (IPL): Investigating language comprehension in typically developing toddlers and young children with autism. Journal of Visualized Experiments: JoVE, 70, e4331. doi:10.3791/4331
Park, C. J., Yelland, G. W., Taffe, J. R., & Gray, K. M. (2012). Brief report: The relationship between language skills, adaptive behaviour, and emotional and behaviour problems in pre-schoolers with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(12), 2761–2766.
R Core Team. (2012). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. ISBN 3-900051-07-0. http://www.R-project.org/
Rapin, I., & Dunn, M. (2003). Update on the language disorders of individuals on the autistic spectrum. Brain and Development, 25(3), 166–172.
Rescorla, L. (2002). Language and reading outcomes to age 9 in late-talking toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 45, 360–371.
Roberts, J. E., Mirrett, P., & Burchinal, M. (2001). Receptive and expressive communication development of young males with fragile X syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 106(3), 216–230.
Roberts, J. A., Rice, M. L., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). Tense marking in children with autism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 429–448.
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). The Social Communication Questionnaire: Manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
Sorace, A., & Keller, F. (2005). Gradience in linguistic data. Lingua, 115(11), 1497–1524.
Swensen, L. D., Kelley, E., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. R. (2007). Processes of language acquisition in children with autism: Evidence from preferential looking. Child Development, 78(2), 542–557.
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1981). On the nature of linguistic functioning in early infantile autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11(1), 45–56.
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1985). Sentence comprehension in autistic children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 2, 5–24.
Tager-Flusberg, H., Calkins, S., Nolin, T., Baumberger, T., Anderson, M., & Chadwick-Dias, A. (1990). A longitudinal study of language acquisition in autistic and Down syndrome children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(1), 1–21.
Tager-Flusberg, H., Lord, C., & Paul, R. (1997). Language and Communication in autism. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive development disorders (2nd ed., pp. 335–365). New York: Wiley.
Tek, S., Mesite, L., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. (2014). Longitudinal analyses of expressive language development reveal two distinct language profiles among young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(1), 75–89.
Thurm, A., Lord, C., Lee, L., & Newschaffer, C. (2007). Predictors of language acquisition in preschool children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1721–1734.
Tomblin, B. (2011). Co-morbidity of autism and SLI: Kinds, kin and complexity. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 46(2), 127–137.
Tomblin, J. B., Hafeman, L. L., & O’Brien, M. (2003). Autism and autism risk in siblings of children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 38(3), 235–250.
Van der Lely, H. K. (1996). Specifically language impaired and normally developing children: Verbal passive vs. adjectival passive sentence interpretation. Lingua, 98(4), 243–272.
Wechsler, D. (2004). The Wechsler Intelligence scale for children (4th ed.). London: Pearson Assessment.
Weismer, S. E., Lord, C., & Esler, A. (2010). Early language patterns of toddlers on the autism spectrum compared to toddlers with developmental delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 1259–1273.
Wexler, K., & Rice, M. (1996). Toward tense as a clinical marker of specific language impairment in English-speaking children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 1239–1257.
Whitehouse, A. J. O., Barry, J. G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2007). The broader language phenotype of autism: A comparison with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 822–830.
Whitehouse, A. J. O., Barry, J. G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2008). Further defining the language impairment of autism: Is there a specific language impairment subtype? Journal of Communication Disorders, 41, 319–336.
Williams, D., Botting, N., & Boucher, J. (2008). Language in autism and specific language impairment: Where are the links? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 944–963.
Wulfeck, B. (1993). A reaction time study of grammaticality judgements in children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 1208–1215.
Wulfeck, B., Bates, E., Krupa-Kwiatkowski, M., & Saltzman, D. (2004). Grammaticality sensitivity in children with early focal brain injury and children with specific language impairment. Brain and Language, 88(2), 215–228.
Acknowledgments
We thank the children, parents and teachers who made this research possible. This study was funded by a grant from the Leverhulme Trust (RPG-158). Ben Ambridge and Colin Bannard are Reader and Lecturer in Psychology in the International Centre for Language and Communicative Development (LuCiD) at The University of Liverpool. The support of the Economic and Social Research Council [ES/L008955/1] is gratefully acknowledged.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Appendices
Appendix 1: Test Sentences
Type | Verb | Intransitive sentence | Transitive sentence |
---|---|---|---|
Counterbalance Group A | |||
ALT | Break | The plate broke into pieces | Homer broke the plate into pieces |
ALT | Grow | The flowers grew in the greenhouse | Homer grew the flowers in the greenhouse |
ALT | Cook | The bread cooked in the oven | Homer cooked the bread in the oven |
ALT | Slide | The truck slid across the floor | Bart slid the truck across the floor |
ALT | Shatter | The vase shattered into pieces | Bart shattered the vase into pieces |
INT | Fall | The cup fell off the shelf | *Lisa fell the cup off the shelf |
INT | Arrive | The children arrived at the party | *Marge arrived the children at the party |
INT | Laugh | The audience laughed at the joke | *Bart laughed the audience at the joke |
INT | Chuckle | The audience chuckled in anticipation | *Bart chuckled the audience in anticipation |
INT | Appear | The coin appeared out of thin air | *Homer appeared the coin out of thin air |
Counterbalance Group B | |||
ALT | Rip | The dress ripped at the seam | Marge ripped the dress at the seam |
ALT | Smash | The glass smashed into bits | Lisa smashed the glass into bits |
ALT | Fold | The scarf folded double | Marge folded the scarf double |
ALT | Open | The door opened in the hallway | Homer opened the door in the hallway |
ALT | Bounce | The ball bounced down the street | Lisa bounced the ball down the street |
INT | Go | The bus went along the pavement | *Homer went the bus along the pavement |
INT | Tumble | The books tumbled off the table | *Homer tumbled the books off the table |
INT | Swim | The fish swam in the tank | *Homer swam the fish in the tank |
INT | Disappear | The money disappeared from the bank account | *Marge disappeared the money from the bank account |
INT | Vanish | The card vanished into thin air | *Bart vanished the card into thin air |
Appendix 2: Training Sentences, and Expected Ratings on the 5-Point Scale
-
The cat drank the milk (5)
-
*The dog the ball played with (1)
-
The frog caught the fly (5)
-
*His teeth man the brushed (1)
-
*The woman said the man a funny story (2–3)
-
*The girl telephoned her friend the news (3–4)
-
The man whispered his friend the joke (4–5)
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Ambridge, B., Bannard, C. & Jackson, G.H. Is Grammar Spared in Autism Spectrum Disorder? Data from Judgments of Verb Argument Structure Overgeneralization Errors. J Autism Dev Disord 45, 3288–3296 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2487-5
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2487-5