Abstract
Here we describe the performance of children with autism, their siblings, and typically developing children using the Florida Apraxia Battery. Children with autism showed the lowest performance in all sections of the test. They were mostly impaired in pantomime actions execution on imitation and on verbal command, and in imitation of meaningless gestures. Interestingly, a correlation was found between performance in pantomime actions and the severity of social behavior deficits. We conclude that the presence of a rigid internal model prevents the execution of an exact copy of the observed pantomime actions and that the deficit in imitation of meaningless gestures is most likely due to a deficit in the mechanisms responsible for visuomotor transformations.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Baranek, G. T. (1999). Autism during infancy: A retrospective video analysis of sensory-motor and social behaviors at 9–12 months of age. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(3), 213–224.
Bhat, A. N., Galloway, J. C., & Landa, R. J. (2012). Relation between early motor delay and later communication delay in infants at risk for autism. Infant Behavior and Development, 35(4), 838–846. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.07.019.
Cattaneo, L., Fabbri-Destro, M., Boria, S., Pieraccini, C., Monti, A., Cossu, G., & Rizzolatti, G. (2007). Impairment of actions chains in autism and its possible role in intention understanding. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(45), 17825–17830. doi:10.1073/pnas.0706273104.
Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9(1), 28–30. doi:10.1038/nn1611.
De Renzi, E., & Faglioni, P. (1999). Apraxia. In G. Denes & L. Pizzamiglio (Eds.), Handbook of clinical and experimental neuropsychology (pp. 421–440). Hove (UK): Psychology Press.
DeMeyer, M. K., Alpern, G. D., Barton, S., DeMyer, W. E., Churchill, D. W., Hingtgen, J. N., et al. (1972). Imitation in autistic, early schizophrenic, and non-psychotic subnormal children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2(3), 264–287.
Dowell, L. R., Mahone, E. M., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2009). Associations of postural knowledge and basic motor skill with dyspraxia in autism: Implication for abnormalities in distributed connectivity and motor learning. Neuropsychology, 23(5), 563–570. doi:10.1037/a0015640.
Dziuk, M. A., Gidley Larson, J. C., Apostu, A., Mahone, E. M., Denckla, M. B., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2007). Dyspraxia in autism: Association with motor, social, and communicative deficits. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 49(10), 734–739. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00734.x.
Gibbs, J., Appleton, J., & Appleton, R. (2007). Dyspraxia or developmental coordination disorder? Unravelling the enigma. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 92(6), 534–539. doi:10.1136/adc.2005.088054.
Gidley Larson, J. C., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2006). Motor deficits in autism. In R. Tuchman & I. Rapin (Eds.), Autism: A neurological disorder of early brain development (pp. 231–247). London: MacKeith Press.
Gizzonio, V., Avanzini, P., Fabbri-Destro, M., Campi, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2014). Cognitive abilities in siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders. Experimental Brain Research, 232(7), 2381–2390. doi:10.1007/s00221-014-3935-8.
Gonzales Rothi, L. J., Raymer, A. M., & Heilman, K. M. (1997). Limb praxis assessment. In L. J. Gonzales Rothi & K. M. Heilman (Eds.), Apraxia: The cognitive neuropsychology of praxis. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Gowen, E., & Hamilton, A. (2013). Motor abilities in autism: A review using a computational context. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(2), 323–344. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1574-0.
Haas, R. H., Townsend, J., Courchesne, E., Lincoln, A. J., Schreibman, L., & Yeung-Courchesne, R. (1996). Neurologic abnormalities in infantile autism. Journal of Child Neurology, 11(2), 84–92.
Hallett, M., Lebiedowska, M. K., Thomas, S. L., Stanhope, S. J., Denckla, M. B., & Rumsey, J. (1993). Locomotion of autistic adults. Archives of Neurology, 50(12), 1304–1308.
Heilman, K. M., Rothi, L. J., & Valenstein, E. (1982). Two forms of ideomotor apraxia. Neurology, 32(4), 342–346. doi:10.1212/WNL.32.4.342.
Hobson, R. P., & Hobson, J. A. (2008). Dissociable aspects of imitation: A study in autism. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101(3), 170–185. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.04.007.
Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1999). Imitation and identification in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(4), 649–659.
Jones, V., & Prior, M. (1985). Motor imitation abilities and neurological signs in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 15(1), 37–46.
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.
Lord, C., Rutter, M., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J., Jordan, H., Mawhood, L., & Schopler, E. (1989). Autism diagnostic observation schedule: A standardized observation of communicative and social behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19(2), 185–212.
MacNeil, L. K., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2012). Specificity of dyspraxia in children with autism. Neuropsychology, 26(2), 165–171. doi:10.1037/a0026955.
Mostofsky, S. H., Dubey, P., Jerath, V. K., Jansiewicz, E. M., Goldberg, M. C., & Denckla, M. B. (2006). Developmental dyspraxia is not limited to imitation in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS, 12(3), 314–326.
Mozaz, M., Rothi, L. J. G., Anderson, J. M., Crucian, G. P., & Heilman, K. M. (2002). Postural knowledge of transitive pantomimes and intransitive gestures. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS, 8(7), 958–962.
Nebel, M. B., Eloyan, A., Barber, A. D., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2014a). Precentral gyrus functional connectivity signatures of autism. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 8, 80. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2014.00080.
Nebel, M. B., Joel, S. E., Muschelli, J., Barber, A. D., Caffo, B. S., Pekar, J. J., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2014b). Disruption of functional organization within the primary motor cortex in children with autism. Human Brain Mapping, 35(2), 567–580. doi:10.1002/hbm.22188.
Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(2), 190–198. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.014.
Orsini, L., & Picone, S. (2006). Weschsler intelligence scale for children—III Edizione Italiana. Firenze: Giunti OS.
Piaget, J. (1962). The stages of the intellectual development of the child. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 26, 120–128.
Power, E., Code, C., Croot, K., Sheard, C., & Gonzalez-Rothi, L. J. (2010). Florida apraxia battery-extended and revised sydney (FABERS): Design, description, and a healthy control sample. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(1), 1–18. doi:10.1080/13803390902791646.
Rapin, I. (1991). Autistic children: Diagnosis and clinical features. Pediatrics, 87(5 Pt 2), 751–760.
Rizzolatti, G., Cattaneo, L., Fabbri-Destro, M., & Rozzi, S. (2014). Cortical mechanisms underlying the organization of goal-directed actions and mirror neuron-based action understanding. Physiological Reviews, 94(2), 655–706. doi:10.1152/physrev.00009.2013.
Rogers, S. J., Bennetto, L., McEvoy, R., & Pennington, B. F. (1996). Imitation and pantomime in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Child Development, 67(5), 2060–2073.
Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3(02), 137–162. doi:10.1017/S0954579400000043.
Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. NewYork: BasicBooks.
Sutera, S., Pandey, J., Esser, E. L., Rosenthal, M. A., Wilson, L. B., Barton, M., et al. (2007). Predictors of optimal outcome in toddlers diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(1), 98–107. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0340-6.
Teitelbaum, P., Teitelbaum, O., Nye, J., Fryman, J., & Maurer, R. G. (1998). Movement analysis in infancy may be useful for early diagnosis of autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95(23), 13982–13987.
Vilensky, J. A., Damasio, A. R., & Maurer, R. G. (1981). Gait disturbances in patients with autistic behavior: A preliminary study. Archives of Neurology, 38(10), 646–649.
Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler intelligence scale for children—Third edition manual. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25(4), 287–295.
Acknowledgments
The work was supported by the ERC Project No. 250013 funded to GR. The authors would like to thank Dr. Costanza Carmi for the English correction.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Gizzonio, V., Avanzini, P., Campi, C. et al. Failure in Pantomime Action Execution Correlates with the Severity of Social Behavior Deficits in Children with Autism: A Praxis Study. J Autism Dev Disord 45, 3085–3097 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2461-2
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2461-2