Skip to main content
Log in

Approximating Implicit and Explicit Mentalizing with Two Naturalistic Video-Based Tasks in Typical Development and Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have been proposed to show greater impairments in implicit than explicit mentalizing. To test this proposition, we developed two comparable naturalistic tasks for a performance-based approximation of implicit and explicit mentalizing in 28 individuals with ASD and 23 matched typically developed (TD) participants. Although both tasks were sensitive to the social impairments of individuals with ASD, implicit mentalizing was not more dysfunctional than explicit mentalizing. In TD participants, performance on the tasks did not correlate with each other, whereas in individuals with ASD they were highly correlated. These findings suggest that implicit and explicit mentalizing processes are separable in typical development. In contrast, in individuals with ASD implicit and explicit mentalizing processes are similarly impaired and closely linked suggesting a lack of developmental specification of these processes in ASD.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adolphs, R. (2009). The social brain: Neural basis of social knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 693–716. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163514.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Apperly, I. A., & Butterfill, S. A. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review, 116(4), 953–970. doi:10.1037/a0016923.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Auyeung, B., Allison, C., Wheelwright, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2012). Brief report: Development of the adolescent empathy and systemizing quotients. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(10), 2225–2235. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1454-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., & He, Z. (2010). False-belief understanding in infants. Trends Cogn Sci, 14(3), 110–118.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Theory of mind and autism: A review. International Review of Mental Retardation, 23, 169–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(7), 813–822.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Richler, J., Bisarya, D., Gurunathan, N., & Wheelwright, S. (2003). The systemizing quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 358(1430), 361–374. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1206.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 241–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, S. L., & Seltzer, M. M. (2012). Self-reported autism symptoms in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(11), 2354–2363. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1483-2.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boelte, S., Duketis, E., Poustka, F., & Holtmann, M. (2011). Sex differences in cognitive domains and their clinical correlates in higher-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 15(4), 497–511. doi:10.1177/13623613103911161362361310391116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brackett, M. A., Rivers, S. E., Shiffman, S., Lerner, N., & Salovey, P. (2006). Relating emotional abilities to social functioning: A comparison of self-report and performance measures of emotional intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(4), 780–795. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.780.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Callenmark, B., Kjellin, L., Ronnqvist, L., & Boelte, S. (2013). Explicit versus implicit social cognition testing in autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 18(6), 684–693.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, A. S., Black, D. O., Tewani, S., Connolly, C. E., Kadlec, M. B., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2007). Sex differences in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(1), 86–97. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0331-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125(8), 1839–1849.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33(5), 781–789.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clements, W. A., Rustin, C. L., & McCallum, S. (2000). Promoting the transition from implicit to explicit understanding: A training study of false belief. Developmental Science, 3(1), 81–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, W. A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). Attitudes and evaluations: A social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Trends Cogn Sci, 11(3), 97–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, W. A., Zelazo, P. D., Packer, D. J., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2007). The iterative reprocessing model: A multilevel framework for attitudes and evaluation. Social Cognition, 25(5), 736–760.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J., & Moors, A. (2010). Implicit measures: Similarities and differences. In B. Gawronski & B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dziobek, I., Fleck, S., Kalbe, E., Rogers, K., Hassenstab, J., Brand, M., et al. (2006). Introducing MASC: A movie for the assessment of social cognition. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(5), 623–636. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0107-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition. Research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145225101601.145225.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C. D. (2004). Schizophrenia and theory of mind. Psychological Medicine, 34(3), 385–389.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2012). Mechanisms of social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 287–313. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100449.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gillberg, C., Rastam, M., & Wentz, E. (2001). The Asperger syndrome (and high-functioning autism) diagnostic interview (ASDI): A preliminary study of a new structured clinical interview. Autism, 5(1), 57–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golan, O., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Systemizing empathy: Teaching adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism to recognize complex emotions using interactive multimedia. Development and Psychopathology, 18(2), 591–617. doi:10.1017/S0954579406060305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hill, J. (2006). The Cambridge mindreading (CAM) face-voice battery: Testing complex emotion recognition in adults with and without Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(2), 169–183. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0057-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., Hill, J. J., & Rutherford, M. D. (2007). The ‘Reading the Mind in the Voice’ test-revised: A study of complex emotion recognition in adults with and without autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(6), 1096–1106. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0252-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hadwin, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Howlin, P., & Hill, K. (1997). Does teaching theory of mind have an effect on the ability to develop conversation in children with autism? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(5), 519–537.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. G. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48(2), 101–119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. G. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. G., & Frith, U. (1996). The neuropsychology of autism. Brain, 119(Pt 4), 1377–1400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, E. L., & Frith, U. (2003). Understanding autism: Insights from mind and brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 358(1430), 281–289. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1209.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2009). Heterogeneity and homogeneity across the autism spectrum: The role of development. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(5), 471–473.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ketelaars, C., Horwitz, E., Sytema, S., Bos, J., Wiersma, D., Minderaa, R., et al. (2008). Brief report: Adults with mild autism spectrum disorders (ASD): Scores on the autism spectrum quotient (AQ) and comorbid psychopathology. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(1), 176–180. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0358-4.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchner, J. C., Hatri, A., Heekeren, H. R., & Dziobek, I. (2011). Autistic symptomatology, face processing abilities, and eye fixation patterns. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(2), 158–167.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kliemann, D., Dziobek, I., Hatri, A., Steimke, R., & Heekeren, H. R. (2010). Atypical reflexive gaze patterns on emotional faces in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(37), 12281–12287. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0688-10.2010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kliemann, D., Rosenblau, G., Boelte, S., Heekeren, H. R., & Dziobek, I. (2013). Face puzzle-two new video-based tasks for measuring explicit and implicit aspects of facial emotion recognition. Front Psychol, 4, 376. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00376.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(9), 809–816.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Lin, D.J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., & Jones, W. (2009). Two-year-olds with autism fail to orient towards human biological motion but attend instead to non-social, physical contingencies. Nature, 459, 257–261. Klin.

  • Kovács, A. M., Teglas, E., & Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense: Susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults. Science, 330(6012), 1830–1834. doi:10.1126/science.1190792.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M. V., Pasco, G., Ruigrok, A. N., Wheelwright, S. J., Sadek, S. A., et al. (2011). A behavioral comparison of male and female adults with high functioning autism spectrum conditions. PLoS ONE, 6(6), e20835. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0020835PONE-D-11-03038.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lehrl, S. (1989). Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest [multiple-choice vocabulary test]. Nürnberg, Germany: Medizinische Verlagsgesellschaft.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., Dilavore, P., & Risi, S. (2002). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. Los Angeles: CA: Western Psychological Services.

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Schopler, E., & Revicki, D. (1982). Sex differences in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 12(4), 317–330.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Low, J., & Perner, J. (2012). Implicit and explicit theory of mind: State of the art. Br J Dev Psychol, 30(Pt 1), 1–13. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02074.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moran, J. M., Young, L. L., Saxe, R., Lee, S. M., O’Young, D., Mavros, P. L., et al. (2011). Impaired theory of mind for moral judgment in high-functioning autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 108(7), 2688–2692. doi:10.1073/pnas.10117341081011734108.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A., Hawkins, C. B., & Frazier, R. S. (2011). Implicit social cognition: From measures to mechanisms. Trends Cogn Sci, 15(4), 152–159. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.01.005.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308(5719), 255–258. doi:10.1126/science.1107621.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paulus, C. (2006). Der Saarbrücker Persönlichkeitsfragebogen SPF (IRI).http://www.uni-saarland.de/fak5/ezw/personal/paulus/empathy/SPF(IRI)_V5.0.pdf.

  • Pitskel, N. B., Bolling, D. Z., Hudac, C. M., Lantz, S. D., Minshew, N. J., Vander wyk, B. C., et al. (2011). Brain mechanisms for processing direct and averted gaze in individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(12), 1686–1693.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Chimpanzee problem-solving: A test for comprehension. Science, 202(4367), 532–535.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruffman, T., Garnham, W., Import, A., & Connolly, D. (2001). Does eye gaze indicate implicit knowledge of false belief? Charting transitions in knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80(3), 201–224. doi:10.1006/jecp.2001.2633S0022-0965(01)92633-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Samson, D., Apperly, I. A., Braithwaite, J. J., Andrews, B. J., & Bodley Scott, S. E. (2010). Seeing it their way: Evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(5), 1255–1266. doi:10.1037/a0018729.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, R., & Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people. The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”. Neuroimage, 19(4), 1835–1842.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, D., Slaughter, V. P., Bayliss, A. P., & Dux, P. E. (2013). A temporally sustained implicit theory of mind deficit in autism spectrum disorders. Cognition, 129(2), 410–417.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A. (2013). Atypical development of spontaneous social cognition in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Development, 35(2), 96–101. doi:10.1016/j.braindev.2012.08.002S0387-7604(12)00197-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science, 325(5942), 883–885. doi:10.1126/science.1176170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sharp, C., & Venta, A. (2012). Mentalizing problems in children and adolescents. In N. Midgley & I. Vrouva (Eds.), Minding the child: Mentalization-based interventions with children, young people and their families. London, UK: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sucksmith, E., Allison, C., Baron-Cohen, S., Chakrabarti, B., & Hoekstra, R. A. (2013). Empathy and emotion recognition in people with autism, first-degree relatives, and controls. Neuropsychologia, 51(1), 98–105. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.11.013S0028-3932(12)00480-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Charman, T., Cox, A., Baird, G., Drew, A., et al. (1998). The frequency and distribution of spontaneous attention shifts between social and nonsocial stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and nonautistic developmentally delayed infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(5), 747–753.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thoermer, C., Sodian, B., Vuori, M., Perst, H., & Kristen, S. (2012). Continuity from an implicit to an explicit understanding of false belief from infancy to preschool age. Br J Dev Psychol, 30(1), 172–187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vierkant, T. (2012). Self-knowledge and knowing other minds: The implicit/explicit distinction as a tool in understanding theory of mind. Br J Dev Psychol, 30(1), 141–155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Volkmar, F. R., Lord, C., Bailey, A., Schultz, R. T., & Klin, A. (2004). Autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(1), 135–170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yoshida, W., Dziobek, I., Kliemann, D., Heekeren, H. R., Friston, K. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2010). Cooperation and heterogeneity of the autistic mind. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(26), 8815–8818. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0400-10.2010.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zaki, J., & Ochsner, K. (2009). The need for a cognitive neuroscience of naturalistic social cognition. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1167, 16–30. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04601.x.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zaki, J., & Ochsner, K. (2011). Reintegrating the study of accuracy into social cognition research. Psychological Inquiry, 22(3), 159–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Joerg Schulze, member of the Computer and Media Service team (CMS) at the Humboldt University, for his advice and support with the production and postproduction of the film stimuli and the actors. We also thank our student research assistants for their help with the stimulus production and data acquisition. In addition, we would like especially our participants on the autism spectrum for their participation in the study. This work was supported by a Grant from the German Research Foundation (DFG; EXC 302).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gabriela Rosenblau.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOC 32 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rosenblau, G., Kliemann, D., Heekeren, H.R. et al. Approximating Implicit and Explicit Mentalizing with Two Naturalistic Video-Based Tasks in Typical Development and Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 45, 953–965 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2249-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2249-9

Keywords

Navigation