Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 45, Issue 4, pp 953–965 | Cite as

Approximating Implicit and Explicit Mentalizing with Two Naturalistic Video-Based Tasks in Typical Development and Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • Gabriela Rosenblau
  • Dorit Kliemann
  • Hauke R. Heekeren
  • Isabel Dziobek
Original Paper

Abstract

Individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have been proposed to show greater impairments in implicit than explicit mentalizing. To test this proposition, we developed two comparable naturalistic tasks for a performance-based approximation of implicit and explicit mentalizing in 28 individuals with ASD and 23 matched typically developed (TD) participants. Although both tasks were sensitive to the social impairments of individuals with ASD, implicit mentalizing was not more dysfunctional than explicit mentalizing. In TD participants, performance on the tasks did not correlate with each other, whereas in individuals with ASD they were highly correlated. These findings suggest that implicit and explicit mentalizing processes are separable in typical development. In contrast, in individuals with ASD implicit and explicit mentalizing processes are similarly impaired and closely linked suggesting a lack of developmental specification of these processes in ASD.

Keywords

Autism spectrum disorder Cognition Mentalizing Behavioral assessment Implicit 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We thank Joerg Schulze, member of the Computer and Media Service team (CMS) at the Humboldt University, for his advice and support with the production and postproduction of the film stimuli and the actors. We also thank our student research assistants for their help with the stimulus production and data acquisition. In addition, we would like especially our participants on the autism spectrum for their participation in the study. This work was supported by a Grant from the German Research Foundation (DFG; EXC 302).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Supplementary material

10803_2014_2249_MOESM1_ESM.doc (32 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOC 32 kb)

References

  1. Adolphs, R. (2009). The social brain: Neural basis of social knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 693–716. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163514.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Apperly, I. A., & Butterfill, S. A. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review, 116(4), 953–970. doi: 10.1037/a0016923.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Auyeung, B., Allison, C., Wheelwright, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2012). Brief report: Development of the adolescent empathy and systemizing quotients. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(10), 2225–2235. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1454-7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., & He, Z. (2010). False-belief understanding in infants. Trends Cogn Sci, 14(3), 110–118.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Theory of mind and autism: A review. International Review of Mental Retardation, 23, 169–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(7), 813–822.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Baron-Cohen, S., Richler, J., Bisarya, D., Gurunathan, N., & Wheelwright, S. (2003). The systemizing quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 358(1430), 361–374. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1206.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 241–251.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Bishop, S. L., & Seltzer, M. M. (2012). Self-reported autism symptoms in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(11), 2354–2363. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1483-2.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Boelte, S., Duketis, E., Poustka, F., & Holtmann, M. (2011). Sex differences in cognitive domains and their clinical correlates in higher-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 15(4), 497–511. doi: 10.1177/13623613103911161362361310391116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Brackett, M. A., Rivers, S. E., Shiffman, S., Lerner, N., & Salovey, P. (2006). Relating emotional abilities to social functioning: A comparison of self-report and performance measures of emotional intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(4), 780–795. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.780.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Callenmark, B., Kjellin, L., Ronnqvist, L., & Boelte, S. (2013). Explicit versus implicit social cognition testing in autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 18(6), 684–693.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Carter, A. S., Black, D. O., Tewani, S., Connolly, C. E., Kadlec, M. B., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2007). Sex differences in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(1), 86–97. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0331-7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125(8), 1839–1849.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33(5), 781–789.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Clements, W. A., Rustin, C. L., & McCallum, S. (2000). Promoting the transition from implicit to explicit understanding: A training study of false belief. Developmental Science, 3(1), 81–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Cunningham, W. A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). Attitudes and evaluations: A social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Trends Cogn Sci, 11(3), 97–104.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Cunningham, W. A., Zelazo, P. D., Packer, D. J., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2007). The iterative reprocessing model: A multilevel framework for attitudes and evaluation. Social Cognition, 25(5), 736–760.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. De Houwer, J., & Moors, A. (2010). Implicit measures: Similarities and differences. In B. Gawronski & B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  21. Dziobek, I., Fleck, S., Kalbe, E., Rogers, K., Hassenstab, J., Brand, M., et al. (2006). Introducing MASC: A movie for the assessment of social cognition. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(5), 623–636. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0107-0.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition. Research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145225101601.145225.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
  24. Frith, C. D. (2004). Schizophrenia and theory of mind. Psychological Medicine, 34(3), 385–389.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2012). Mechanisms of social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 287–313. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100449.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Gillberg, C., Rastam, M., & Wentz, E. (2001). The Asperger syndrome (and high-functioning autism) diagnostic interview (ASDI): A preliminary study of a new structured clinical interview. Autism, 5(1), 57–66.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Golan, O., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Systemizing empathy: Teaching adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism to recognize complex emotions using interactive multimedia. Development and Psychopathology, 18(2), 591–617. doi: 10.1017/S0954579406060305.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hill, J. (2006). The Cambridge mindreading (CAM) face-voice battery: Testing complex emotion recognition in adults with and without Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(2), 169–183. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0057-y.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., Hill, J. J., & Rutherford, M. D. (2007). The ‘Reading the Mind in the Voice’ test-revised: A study of complex emotion recognition in adults with and without autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(6), 1096–1106. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0252-5.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Hadwin, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Howlin, P., & Hill, K. (1997). Does teaching theory of mind have an effect on the ability to develop conversation in children with autism? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(5), 519–537.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Happé, F. G. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48(2), 101–119.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Happé, F. G. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129–154.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Happé, F. G., & Frith, U. (1996). The neuropsychology of autism. Brain, 119(Pt 4), 1377–1400.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Hill, E. L., & Frith, U. (2003). Understanding autism: Insights from mind and brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 358(1430), 281–289. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1209.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2009). Heterogeneity and homogeneity across the autism spectrum: The role of development. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(5), 471–473.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Ketelaars, C., Horwitz, E., Sytema, S., Bos, J., Wiersma, D., Minderaa, R., et al. (2008). Brief report: Adults with mild autism spectrum disorders (ASD): Scores on the autism spectrum quotient (AQ) and comorbid psychopathology. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(1), 176–180. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0358-4.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Kirchner, J. C., Hatri, A., Heekeren, H. R., & Dziobek, I. (2011). Autistic symptomatology, face processing abilities, and eye fixation patterns. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(2), 158–167.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Kliemann, D., Dziobek, I., Hatri, A., Steimke, R., & Heekeren, H. R. (2010). Atypical reflexive gaze patterns on emotional faces in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(37), 12281–12287. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0688-10.2010.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Kliemann, D., Rosenblau, G., Boelte, S., Heekeren, H. R., & Dziobek, I. (2013). Face puzzle-two new video-based tasks for measuring explicit and implicit aspects of facial emotion recognition. Front Psychol, 4, 376. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00376.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(9), 809–816.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Klin, A., Lin, D.J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., & Jones, W. (2009). Two-year-olds with autism fail to orient towards human biological motion but attend instead to non-social, physical contingencies. Nature, 459, 257–261. Klin.Google Scholar
  42. Kovács, A. M., Teglas, E., & Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense: Susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults. Science, 330(6012), 1830–1834. doi: 10.1126/science.1190792.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M. V., Pasco, G., Ruigrok, A. N., Wheelwright, S. J., Sadek, S. A., et al. (2011). A behavioral comparison of male and female adults with high functioning autism spectrum conditions. PLoS ONE, 6(6), e20835. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020835PONE-D-11-03038.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Lehrl, S. (1989). Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest [multiple-choice vocabulary test]. Nürnberg, Germany: Medizinische Verlagsgesellschaft.Google Scholar
  45. Lord, C., Rutter, M., Dilavore, P., & Risi, S. (2002). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. Los Angeles: CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
  46. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Lord, C., Schopler, E., & Revicki, D. (1982). Sex differences in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 12(4), 317–330.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Low, J., & Perner, J. (2012). Implicit and explicit theory of mind: State of the art. Br J Dev Psychol, 30(Pt 1), 1–13. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02074.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Moran, J. M., Young, L. L., Saxe, R., Lee, S. M., O’Young, D., Mavros, P. L., et al. (2011). Impaired theory of mind for moral judgment in high-functioning autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 108(7), 2688–2692. doi: 10.1073/pnas.10117341081011734108.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Nosek, B. A., Hawkins, C. B., & Frazier, R. S. (2011). Implicit social cognition: From measures to mechanisms. Trends Cogn Sci, 15(4), 152–159. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.01.005.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308(5719), 255–258. doi: 10.1126/science.1107621.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Paulus, C. (2006). Der Saarbrücker Persönlichkeitsfragebogen SPF (IRI).http://www.uni-saarland.de/fak5/ezw/personal/paulus/empathy/SPF(IRI)_V5.0.pdf.
  53. Pitskel, N. B., Bolling, D. Z., Hudac, C. M., Lantz, S. D., Minshew, N. J., Vander wyk, B. C., et al. (2011). Brain mechanisms for processing direct and averted gaze in individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(12), 1686–1693.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Chimpanzee problem-solving: A test for comprehension. Science, 202(4367), 532–535.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Ruffman, T., Garnham, W., Import, A., & Connolly, D. (2001). Does eye gaze indicate implicit knowledge of false belief? Charting transitions in knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80(3), 201–224. doi: 10.1006/jecp.2001.2633S0022-0965(01)92633-5.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Samson, D., Apperly, I. A., Braithwaite, J. J., Andrews, B. J., & Bodley Scott, S. E. (2010). Seeing it their way: Evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(5), 1255–1266. doi: 10.1037/a0018729.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Saxe, R., & Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people. The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”. Neuroimage, 19(4), 1835–1842.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Schneider, D., Slaughter, V. P., Bayliss, A. P., & Dux, P. E. (2013). A temporally sustained implicit theory of mind deficit in autism spectrum disorders. Cognition, 129(2), 410–417.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Senju, A. (2013). Atypical development of spontaneous social cognition in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Development, 35(2), 96–101. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2012.08.002S0387-7604(12)00197-0.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science, 325(5942), 883–885. doi: 10.1126/science.1176170.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Sharp, C., & Venta, A. (2012). Mentalizing problems in children and adolescents. In N. Midgley & I. Vrouva (Eds.), Minding the child: Mentalization-based interventions with children, young people and their families. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
  62. Sucksmith, E., Allison, C., Baron-Cohen, S., Chakrabarti, B., & Hoekstra, R. A. (2013). Empathy and emotion recognition in people with autism, first-degree relatives, and controls. Neuropsychologia, 51(1), 98–105. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.11.013S0028-3932(12)00480-0.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Charman, T., Cox, A., Baird, G., Drew, A., et al. (1998). The frequency and distribution of spontaneous attention shifts between social and nonsocial stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and nonautistic developmentally delayed infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(5), 747–753.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Thoermer, C., Sodian, B., Vuori, M., Perst, H., & Kristen, S. (2012). Continuity from an implicit to an explicit understanding of false belief from infancy to preschool age. Br J Dev Psychol, 30(1), 172–187.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Vierkant, T. (2012). Self-knowledge and knowing other minds: The implicit/explicit distinction as a tool in understanding theory of mind. Br J Dev Psychol, 30(1), 141–155.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Volkmar, F. R., Lord, C., Bailey, A., Schultz, R. T., & Klin, A. (2004). Autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(1), 135–170.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Yoshida, W., Dziobek, I., Kliemann, D., Heekeren, H. R., Friston, K. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2010). Cooperation and heterogeneity of the autistic mind. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(26), 8815–8818. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0400-10.2010.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Zaki, J., & Ochsner, K. (2009). The need for a cognitive neuroscience of naturalistic social cognition. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1167, 16–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04601.x.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Zaki, J., & Ochsner, K. (2011). Reintegrating the study of accuracy into social cognition research. Psychological Inquiry, 22(3), 159–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Gabriela Rosenblau
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
  • Dorit Kliemann
    • 1
    • 2
    • 4
    • 5
  • Hauke R. Heekeren
    • 1
    • 2
  • Isabel Dziobek
    • 1
    • 6
  1. 1.Cluster of Excellence ‘Languages of Emotion’Freie Universität BerlinBerlinGermany
  2. 2.Department of Education and PsychologyFreie Universität BerlinBerlinGermany
  3. 3.Center for Translational Developmental Neuroscience, Yale Child Study CenterYale UniversityNew HavenUSA
  4. 4.McGovern Institute for Brain Research at Massachusetts Institute of TechnologyCambridgeUSA
  5. 5.Department of NeurologyMassachusetts General Hospital/Harvard Medical SchoolCharlestownUSA
  6. 6.Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt-Universität zu BerlinBerlinGermany

Personalised recommendations