Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 44, Issue 12, pp 3016–3025 | Cite as

Quantifying Narrative Ability in Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Computational Linguistic Analysis of Narrative Coherence

Original Paper

Abstract

Autism is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by serious difficulties with the social use of language, along with impaired social functioning and ritualistic/repetitive behaviors (American Psychiatric Association in Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5, 5th edn. American Psychiatric Association, Arlington, 2013). While substantial heterogeneity exists in symptom expression, impairments in language discourse skills, including narrative (or storytelling), are universally observed in autism (Tager-Flusberg et al. in Handbook on autism and pervasive developmental disorders, 3rd edn. Wiley, New York, pp 335–364, 2005). This study applied a computational linguistic tool, Latent Semantic Analysis (LSA), to objectively characterize narrative performance in high-functioning individuals with autism and typically-developing controls, across two different narrative contexts that differ in the interpersonal and cognitive demands placed on the narrator. Results indicated that high-functioning individuals with autism produced narratives comparable in semantic content to those produced by controls when narrating from a picture book, but produced narratives diminished in semantic quality in a more demanding narrative recall task. This pattern is similar to that detected from analyses of hand-coded picture book narratives in prior research, and extends findings to an additional narrative context that proves particularly challenging for individuals with autism. Results are discussed in terms of the utility of LSA as a quantitative, objective, and efficient measure of narrative ability.

Keywords

Autism Endophenotype Language Narrative Phenotype 

References

  1. Almasy, L., & Blangero, J. (2001). Endophenotypes as quantitative risk factors for psychiatric disease: Rationale and study design. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 105, 42–44.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  3. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  4. Asperger, H. (1991). ‘Autistic psychopathy’ in childhood (U. Frith, Trans.). In U. Frith (Ed.), Autism and Asperger syndrome. London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Black, M., Katsamanis, A., Lee, C.-C., Lammert, A. C., Baucom, B. R., Christensen, A., et al. (2010). Automatic classification of married couples’ behavior using audio features. In 11th annual conference of the International Speech Communication Association 2010 (Interspeech 2010) (Vols. 3, 4, pp. 2030–2033).Google Scholar
  6. Bruner, J. (1986). Acutal minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Bruner, J., & Feldman, C. (1993). Theories of mind and the problem of autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  9. Capps, L., Kehres, J., & Sigman, M. (1998). Conversational abilities among children with autism and developmental delay. Autism, 2, 325–344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Capps, L., Losh, M., & Thurber, C. (2000). The frog ate a bug and made his mouth sad: Narrative competence in children with autism. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 193–204.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dennis, M., Jacennik, B., & Barnes, M. A. (1994). The content of narrative discourse in children and adolescents after early-onset hydrocephalus and in normally developing age peers. Brain and Language, 46(1), 129–165. doi:10.1006/brln.1994.1008.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Diehl, J. J., Bennetto, L., & Young, E. C. (2006). Story recall and narrative coherence of high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34(1), 83–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Elvevag, B., Foltz, P. W., Weinberger, D. R., & Goldberg, T. E. (2007). Quantifying incoherence in speech: An automated methodology and novel application to schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 93(1–3), 304–316. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.03.001.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Firth, J. R. (1957). Papers in linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Foltz, P. W., Kintsch, W., & Landauer, T. K. (1998). The measurement of textual coherence with latent semantic analysis. Discourse Processes, 25, 285–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Franzke, M., Kintsch, E., Caccamise, D., Johnson, N., & Dooley, S. (2005). Summary Street®: Computer support for comprehension and writing. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 33(1), 304–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1999). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 493–500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Georgiou, P. G., Black, M. P., & Narayanan, S. S. (2011). Behavioral signal processing for understanding (distressed) dyadic interactions: Some recent developments. Paper presented at the third international workshop on social signal processing (SSPW’11), ACM Multimedia’11, Scottsdale, AZ.Google Scholar
  19. Goldman, S. (2008). Brief report: Narratives of personal events in children with autism and developmental language disorders: Unshared memories. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(10), 1982–1988. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0588-0.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gottesman, I. I., & Gould, T. D. (2003). The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: Etymology and strategic intentions. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(4), 636–645.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Grandin, T. (1995). Thinking in pictures. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
  22. Jurgens, D., & Stevens, K. (2010). The S-Space package: An open source package for word space models. Paper presented at the proceedings of the ACL 2010 systems demonstrations, Uppsala, Sweden.Google Scholar
  23. Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. The Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.Google Scholar
  24. Kintsch, W. (2002). The potential of latent semantic analysis for machine grading of clinical case summaries. Journal Biomedical Informatics, 35(1), 3–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104(2), 211–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Landauer, T. K., McNamara, D. S., Dennis, S., & Kintsch, W. (Eds.). (2007). Handbook of latent semantic analysis. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  27. Ledoux, K., Camblin, C. C., Swaab, T. Y., & Gordon, P. C. (2006). Reading words in discourse: The modulation of lexical priming effects by message-level context. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 5, 107–127.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P., et al. (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Losh, M., & Capps, L. (2003). Narrative ability in high-functioning children with autism or Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(3), 239–251.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Losh, M., & Capps, L. (2006). Understanding of emotional experience in autism: Insights from the personal accounts of high-functioning children with autism. Developmental Psychology, 42(5), 809–818.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Loveland, K. A., McEvoy, R. E., & Tunali, B. (1990). Narrative story telling in autism and Down’s syndrome. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8, 9–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Loveland, K. A., & Tunali, B. (1993). Narrative language in autism and the theory of mind hypothesis: A wider perspective. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  34. MacWhinney, B. (2007). The TalkBank Project. In J. C. Beal, K. P. Corrigan, & H. L. Moisl (Eds.), Creating and digitizing language corpora: Synchronic databases (Vol. 1). Houndmills: Palgrave-Macmillan.Google Scholar
  35. Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you?. New York: Dial Press.Google Scholar
  36. Metallinou, A., Lee, S., Narayanan, S., & IEEE. (2010, March 14–19). Decision level combination of multiple modalities for recognition and analysis of emotional expression. Paper presented at the 2010 IEEE international conference on acoustics, speech, and signal processing, Dallas, TX.Google Scholar
  37. Opie, I. A., & Opie, P. (1974). The classic fairy tales. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  38. Paul, R., Orlovski, S. M., Marcinko, H. C., & Volkmar, F. (2009). Conversational behaviors in youth with high-functioning ASD and asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 115–125.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Reichow, B., Salamack, S., Paul, R., Volkmar, F. R., & Klin, A. (2008). Pragmatic assessment in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A comparison of a standard measure with parent report. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 29(3), 169–176. doi:10.1177/1525740108318697.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Simmons, R., Gordon, P. C., & Chambless, D. (2005). Pronoun use in marital interaction: What do “you” and “I” say about marital health? Psychological Science, 16, 932–936.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Sykes, N. H., & Lamb, J. A. (2007). Autism: The quest for the genes. Expert Reviews in Molecular Medicine, 9(24), 1–15.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Tager-Flusberg, H. (2000). Understanding the language and communicative impairments in autism. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 23, 185–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Tager-Flusberg, H. (2005). Designing studies to investigate the relationships between genes, environments, and developmental language disorders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 29–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Tager-Flusberg, H., & Anderson, M. (1991). The development of contingent discourse ability in autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 1123–1134.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Tager-Flusberg, H., & Caronna, E. (2007). Language disorders: Autism and other pervasive developmental disorders. Pediatric Clinics of North America, 54(3), 469–481. doi:10.1016/j.pcl.2007.02.011.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Tager-Flusberg, H., Paul, R., & Lord, C. (2005). Language and communication in autism. In F. Volkmar, R. Paul, & A. Klin (Eds.), Handbook on autism and pervasive developmental disorders (3rd ed., pp. 335–364). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  47. Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (1995). Attributing mental states to story characters: A comparison of narratives produced by autistic and mentally retarded individuals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16(3), 241–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Turney, P. D., & Pantel, P. (2010). From frequency to meaning: Vector space models of semantics. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research, 37, 141–188.Google Scholar
  49. Volden, J., Coolican, J., Garon, N., White, J., & Bryson, S. (2009). Brief report: Pragmatic language in autism spectrum disorder: Relationships to measures of ability and disability. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(2), 388–393. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0618-y.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Wechsler, D. (2003). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fourth edition. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  51. Williams, J. (1978). The practical princess and other liberating fairy tales. New York: Parents’ Magazine Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Roxelyn and Richard Pepper Department of Communication Sciences and DisordersNorthwestern UniversityEvanstonUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of North CarolinaChapel HillUSA

Personalised recommendations