Vocal Patterns in Infants with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Canonical Babbling Status and Vocalization Frequency
- 1.8k Downloads
- 38 Citations
Abstract
Canonical babbling is a critical milestone for speech development and is usually well in place by 10 months. The possibility that infants with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) show late onset of canonical babbling has so far eluded evaluation. Rate of vocalization or “volubility” has also been suggested as possibly aberrant in infants with ASD. We conducted a retrospective video study examining vocalizations of 37 infants at 9–12 and 15–18 months. Twenty-three of the 37 infants were later diagnosed with ASD and indeed produced low rates of canonical babbling and low volubility by comparison with the 14 typically developing infants. The study thus supports suggestions that very early vocal patterns may prove to be a useful component of early screening and diagnosis of ASD.
Keywords
Canonical babbling Volubility Vocal patterns Early detectionNotes
Acknowledgments
This research was made possible through a grant from the National Institute for Child Health and Human Development (R01-HD42168) and a Grant from Cure Autism Now Foundation (Sensory-Motor and Social-Communicative Symptoms of Autism in Infancy). We thank the families whose participation made this study possible and the staff who collected and processed data for this project.
References
- American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
- Baranek, G. T. (1999). Autism during infancy: A retrospective video analysis of sensory-motor and social behaviors at 9–12 months of age. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 213–224.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Baranek, G. T., Watson, L. R., Boyd, B. A., Poe, M. D., David, F. J., & McGuire, L. (2013). Hyporesponsiveness to social and nonsocial sensory stimuli in children with autism, children with developmental delays, and typically developing children. Development and Psychopathology, 25(2013), 307–320.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bleile, K. M., Stark, R. E., & McGowan, J. S. (1993). Speech development in a child after decannulation: Further evidence that babbling facilitates later speech development. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 7, 319–337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Center for Disease Control and Prevention. (2012). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders—autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 14 Sites, United States, 2008. Morbidity and mortality weekly report surveillance summaries, 61 1–19. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/ss6103a1.htm.
- Chapman, K., Hardin-Jones, M., Schulte, J., & Halter, K. (2001). Vocal development of 9 months-old babies with cleft palate. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 44, 1268–1283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Chawarska, K., Klin, A., Paul, R., Macari, S., & Volkmar, F. (2009). A prospective study of toddlers with ASD: Short-term diagnostic and cognitive outcomes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50(10), 1235–1245.Google Scholar
- Clement, C. J. (2004). Development of vocalizations in deaf and normally hearing infants. (Ph.D. dissertation), Netherlands Graduate School of Linguistics, Amsterdam.Google Scholar
- Cobo-Lewis, A. B., Oller, D. K., Lynch, M. P., & Levine, S. L. (1996). Relations of motor and vocal milestones in typically developing infants and infants with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 100, 456–467.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
- Davis, B. L., Morrison, H. M., von Hapsburg, D., & Warner, A. D. (2005). Early vocal patterns in infants with varied hearing levels. Volta Review, 105(1), 17–27.Google Scholar
- Delgado, C. E. F., Messinger, D. S., & Yale, M. E. (2002). Infant responses to direction of parental gaze: A comparison of two still-face conditions. Infant Behavior and Development, 25(3), 311–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Eilers, R. E., & Oller, D. K. (1994). Infant vocalizations and the early diagnosis of severe hearing impairment. The Journal of Pediatrics, 124(2), 199–203.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Eilers, R. E., Oller, D. K., Levine, S., Basinger, D., Lynch, M. P., & Urbano, R. (1993). The role of prematurity and socioeconomic status in the onset of canonical babbling in infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 16, 297–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Goldstein, M. H., King, A. P., & West, M. J. (2003). Social interaction shapes babbling: Testing parallels between birdsong and speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(13), 8030–8035.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Goldstein, M. H., & Schwade, J. A. (2008). Social feedback to infants’ babbling facilitates rapid phonological learning. Psychological Science, 19, 515–522.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Goldstein, M. H., Schwade, J. A., & Bornstein, M. H. (2009). The value of vocalizing: Five-month-old infants associate their own noncry vocalizations with responses from adults. Child Development, 80, 636–644.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Goldstein, M. H., & West, M. J. (1999). Consistent responses of human mothers to prelinguistic infants: The effect of prelinguistic repertoire size. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113(1), 52–58.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
- Hulit, L. M., & Howard, M. R. (2002). Born to talk: An introduction to speech and language development. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
- Kent, R., Osberger, M. J., Netsell, R., & Hustedde, C. G. (1987). Phonetic development identical twins differing in auditory function. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52, 64–75.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Koopmans-van Beinum, F. J., Clement, C. J., & van den Dikkenberg-Pot, I. (1998). Influence of lack of auditory speech perception on sound productions of deaf infants. Berne, Switzerland: International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development.Google Scholar
- Koopmans-van Beinum, F. J., & van der Stelt, J. M. (1986). Early stages in the development of speech movements. In B. Lindblom & R. Zetterstrom (Eds.), Precursors of early speech (pp. 37–50). New York: Stockton Press.Google Scholar
- Lewedag, V. L. (1995). Patterns of onset of canonical babbling among typically developing infants. (Doctoral dissertation), University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL.Google Scholar
- Locke, J. L., & Pearson, D. (1990). Linguistic significance of babbling: Evidence from a tracheostomized infant. Journal of Child Language, 17, 1–16.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lord, C. (1995). Follow-up of 2-year-olds referred for possible autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 36(8), 1365–1382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism diagnostic observation schedule (ADOS). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
- Lynch, M. P., Oller, D. K., Steffens, M. L., & Buder, E. H. (1995a). Phrasing in prelinguistic vocalizations. Developmental Psychobiology, 28, 3–23.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lynch, M. P., Oller, D. K., Steffens, M. L., Levine, S. L., Basinger, D., & Umbel, V. (1995b). The onset of speech-like vocalizations in infants with Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 100(1), 68–86.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Manjiviona, J., & Prior, M. (1995). Comparison of Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autistic children on a test of motor impairment. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25, 23–29.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Masataka, N. (2001). Why early linguistic milestones are delayed in children with Williams syndrome: Late onset of hand banging as a possible rate-limiting constraint on the emergence of canonical babbling. Developmental Science, 4, 158–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Matson, J. L., Fodstad, J. C., & Dempsey, T. (2009). What symptoms predict the diagnosis of autism or PDD-NOS in infants and toddlers with developmental delays using the Baby and Infant Screen for Autism Traits. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 12(6), 381–388.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Matson, J. L., Mahan, S., Hess, J. A., Fodstad, J. C., & Neal, D. (2010). Convergent validity of the Autism Spectrum Disorder-Diagnostic for Children (ASD-DC) and Childhood Autism Rating Scales (CARS). Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4(4), 633–638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Miller, L. J., Reisman, J. E., McIntosh, D. N., & Simon, J. (2001). An ecological model of sensory modulation: Performance in children with fragile X syndrome, autistic disorder, attention–deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and sensory modulation dysfunction. In S. Smith-Roley, E. I. Blanche, & R. C. Schaaf (Eds.), Understanding the nature of sensory integration with diverse populations (pp. 57–88). San Antonio, TX: Therapy Skill Builders.Google Scholar
- Molemans, I. (2011). Sounds like babbling: A longitudinal investigation of aspects of the prelexical speech repertoire in young children acquiring Dutch: Normally hearing children and hearing impaired children with a cochlear implant (Ph.D.), University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium.Google Scholar
- Molemans, I., Van den Berg, R., Van Severen, L., & Gillis, S. (2011). How to measure the onset of babbling reliably. Journal of Child Language, 39, 1–30.Google Scholar
- Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen Scales of Early Learning (AGS ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
- Nathani, S., Oller, D. K., & Neal, A. R. (2007). On the robustness of vocal development: An examination of infants with moderate-to-severe hearing loss and additional risk factors. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(6), 1425–1444.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Obenchain, P., Menn, L., & Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (1998). Can speech development at 36 months in children with hearing loss be predicted from information available in the second year of life? Volta Review, 100, 149–180.Google Scholar
- Oller, D. K. (1980). The emergence of the sounds of speech in infancy. In G. Yeni-Komshian, J. Kavanagh, & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Child phonology (Vol. 1, pp. 93–112)., Production New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Oller, D. K. (2000). The emergence of the speech capacity. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
- Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. E. (1982). Similarities of babbling in Spanish- and English-learning babies. Journal of Child Language, 9, 565–578.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. E. (1988). The role of audition in infant babbling. Child Development, 59, 441–449.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Oller, D. K., Eilers, R. E., & Basinger, D. (2001). Intuitive identification of infant vocal sounds by parents. Developmental Science, 4, 49–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Oller, D. K., Eilers, R. E., Basinger, D., Steffens, M. L., & Urbano, R. (1995). Extreme poverty and the development of precursors to the speech capacity. First Language, 15, 167–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Oller, D. K., Eilers, R. E., Neal, A. R., & Cobo-Lewis, A. B. (1998). Late onset canonical babbling: A possible early marker of abnormal development. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 103, 249–265.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Oller, D. K., Eilers, R. E., Neal, A. R., & Schwartz, H. K. (1999). Precursors to speech in infancy: The prediction of speech and language disorders. Journal of Communication Disorders, 32, 223–246.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Oller, D. K., Eilers, R. E., Steffens, M. L., Lynch, M. P., & Urbano, R. (1994). Speech-like vocalizations in infancy: An evaluation of potential risk factors. Journal of Child Language, 21, 33–58.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Oller, D. K., Eilers, R. E., Urbano, R., & Cobo-Lewis, A. B. (1997). Development of precursors to speech in infants exposed to two languages. Journal of Child Language, 27, 407–425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Oller, D. K., Niyogi, P., Gray, S., Richards, J. A., Gilkerson, J., Xu, D., et al. (2010). Automated vocal analysis of naturalistic recordings from children with autism, language delay and typical development. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 13354–13359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Osterling, J. A., Dawson, G., & Munson, J. A. (2002). Early recognition of 1-year-old infants with autism spectrum disorder versus mental retardation. Development and Psychopathology, 14(2), 239–251.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ozonoff, S., Iosif, A., Baguio, F., Cook, I. C., Hill, M. M., Hutman, T., et al. (2010). A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(3), 256–266.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Page, J., & Boucher, J. (1998). Motor impairments in children with autistic disorder. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 14(3), 233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Papoušek, M. (1994). Vom ersten Schrei zum ersten Wort: Anfänge der Sprachentwickelung in der vorsprachlichen Kommunikation. Bern: Verlag Hans Huber.Google Scholar
- Paul, R., Augustyn, A., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. R. (2005). Perception and production of prosody by speakers with ASD spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 205–220.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Paul, R., Fuerst, Y., Ramsay, G., Chawarska, K., & Klin, A. (2011). Out of the mouths of babes: Vocal production in infant siblings of children with ASD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(5), 588–598.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Peppe, S., McCann, J., Gibbon, F., O’Hara, A., & Rutherford, M. (2007). Receptive and expressive prosodic ability in children with high-functioning ASD. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 1015–1028.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ramsdell, H. L., Oller, D. K., Buder, E. H., Ethington, C. A., & Chorna, L. (2012). Identification of prelinguistic phonological categories. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 55, 1626–1629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Robins, D. I., Fein, D., Barton, M. I., & Green, J. A. (2001). The modified checklist for autism in toddlers: An initial study investigating the early detection of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 131–144.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Rogers, S. J., & Ozonoff, S. (2005). Annotation: What do we know about sensory dysfunction in autism? A critical review of the empirical evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(12), 1255–1268.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ross, G. S. (1983). Language functioning and speech development of six children receiving tracheostomy in infancy. Journal of Communication Disorders, 15, 95–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Autism diagnostic interview-revised. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
- Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., & Rochen Renner, B. (1992). The Childhood Autism Rating Scale. Lost Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
- Sheinkopf, S. J., Mundy, P., Oller, D. K., & Steffens, M. (2000). Vocal atypicalities of preverbal autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 345–353.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Simon, B. M., Fowler, S. M., & Handler, S. D. (1983). Communication development in young children with long-term tracheostomies: Preliminary report. International Journal of Otorhinolaryngology, 6, 37–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Snow, C. E. (1995). Issues in the study of input: Fine-tuning universality, individual and developmental differences and necessary causes. In B. MacWhinney & P. Fletcher (Eds.), NETwerken: Bijdragen van het vijfde NET symposium: Antwerp Papers in Linguistics 74 (pp. 5–17). Antwerp: University of Antwerp.Google Scholar
- Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
- Stark, R. E. (1980). Stages of speech development in the first year of life. In G. Yeni-Komshian, J. Kavanagh, & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Child phonology (Vol. 1, pp. 73–90). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Stark, R. E., Ansel, B. M., & Bond, J. (1988). Are prelinguistic abilities predictive of learning disability? A follow-up study. In R. L. Masland & M. Masland (Eds.), Preschool prevention of reading failure. Parkton, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
- Stoel-Gammon, C. (1989). Prespeech and early speech development of two late talkers. First Language, 9, 207–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Stoel-Gammon, C. (2011). Relationships between lexical and phonological development in young children. Journal of Child Language, 38(1), 1–34.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Stoel-Gammon, C., & Otomo, K. (1986). Babbling development of hearing impaired and normally hearing subjects. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 51, 33–41.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Teitelbaum, P., Teitelbaum, O., Nye, J., Fryman, J., & Maurer, R. G. (1998). Movement analysis in infancy may be useful for early diagnosis of autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95(23), 13982–13987.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Tronick, E. Z. (1982). Social interchange in infancy. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
- Van den Dikkenberg-Pot, I., Koopmans-van Beinum, F., & Clement, C. (1998). Influence of lack of auditory speech perception of sound productions of deaf infants. Proceedings of the Institute of Phonetic Sciences, University of Amsterdam, 22, 47–60.Google Scholar
- Vihman, M. M. (1996). Phonological development: The origins of language in the child. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
- Volkmar, F. R., & Chawarska, K. (2008). Autism in infants: An update. World Psychiatry, 7(1), 19–21.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Warlaumont, A. S., Oller, D. K., Dale, R., Richards, J. A., Gilkerson, J., & Xu, D. (2010). Vocal interaction dynamics of children with and without autism. In Paper presented at the Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Austin, TX.Google Scholar
- Warren, S. F., Gilkerson, J., Richards, J. A., & Oller, D. K. (2010). What automated vocal analysis reveals about the language learning environment of young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 555–569.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Watson, L. R., Crais, E. R., Baranek, G. T., Dykstra, J. R., & Wilson, K. P. (2013). Communicative gesture use in infants with and without autism: A retrospective home video study. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 22, 25–39.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Watson, L. R., Patten, E., Baranek, G. T., Boyd, B. A., Freuler, A., & Lorenzi, J. (2011). Differential associations between sensory response patterns and language, social, and communication measures in children with autism or other developmental disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 54(6), 1562–1576.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Weismer, S. E., Lord, C., & Esler, A. (2010). Early language patterns of toddlers on the autism spectrum compared to toddlers with developmental delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(10), 1259–1273.PubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Werner, E., Dawson, G., Osterling, J., & Dinno, N. (2000). Brief report: Recognition of autism spectrum disorder before one year of age: A retrospective study based on home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 157–162.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wetherby, A. M., Woods, J., Allen, L., Cleary, J., Dickinson, H., & Lord, C. (2004). Early indicators of ASD spectrum disorders in the second year of life. Journal of ASD and Developmental Disorders, 34, 473–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Yale, M. E., Messinger, D. S., Cobo-Lewis, A. B., Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. E. (1999). An event-based analysis of the coordination of early infant vocalizations and facial actions. Developmental Psychology, 35(2), 505–513.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian, J., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23(2–3), 143–152.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar