Skip to main content
Log in

Broad Autism Phenotype in Typically Developing Children Predicts Performance on an Eye-Tracking Measure of Joint Attention

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We examined visual attention allocation during a set of social videos that are intended to elicit the coordination of attention with another person, compared to a control condition. Deficits in joint attention are a characteristic of young children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Participants included a diverse sample of 50 typically developing school-aged children between 3 and 9 years of age (M = 6:3, SD = 1:8). Results demonstrated that gaze allocation differed significantly between the experimental and control condition. Further, individual differences in gaze allocation were significantly predicted by a parent-report measure evaluating features of the broad autism phenotype. This study contributes to a research program that aims to develop and validate an endophenotype measure of ASD.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abrahams, B. S., & Geschwind, D. H. (2008). Advances in autism genetics: On the threshold of a new neurobiology. Nature Reviews Genetics, 9(5), 341–355.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adolphs, R., Spezio, M. L., Parlier, M., & Piven, J. (2008). Distinct face-processing strategies in parents of autistic children. Current Biology, 18(14), 1090–1093.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, D. K., Lord, C., Risi, S., DiLavore, P. S., Shulman, C., Thurm, A., et al. (2007). Patterns of growth in verbal abilities among children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75(4), 594–604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. Child Development, 55(4), 1278–1289.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, D. A. (1993a). Infants’ ability to consult the speaker for clues to word reference. Journal of Child Language, 20(02), 395–418.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, D. A. (1993b). Early referential understanding: Infants’ ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. Developmental Psychology, 29(5), 832–843.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic-child have a theory of mind? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bölte, S., Poustka, F., & Constantino, J. N. (2008). Assessing autistic traits: Cross-cultural validation of the social responsiveness scale (SRS). Autism Research, 1(6), 354–363.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boraston, Z., & Blakemore, S. J. (2007). The application of eye-tracking technology in the study of autism. Journal of Physiology-London, 581(3), 893–898.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brownell, R. (2000). Expressive one-word picture vocabulary test (3rd ed.). Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4), 176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cassel, T., Messinger, D., Ibanez, L., Haltigan, J., Acosta, S., & Buchman, A. (2007). Early social and emotional communication in the infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders: An examination of the broad phenotype. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(1), 122–132.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15(4), 481–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Constantino, J. N. (2002). The social responsiveness scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Constantino, J. N., Davis, S. A., Todd, R. D., Schindler, M. K., Gross, M. M., Brophy, S. L., et al. (2003). Validation of a brief quantitative measure of autistic traits: Comparison of the social responsiveness scale with the autism diagnostic interview-revised. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(4), 427–433.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Constantino, J. N., Lajonchere, C., Lutz, M., Gray, T., Abbacchi, A., McKenna, K., et al. (2006). Autistic social impairment in the siblings of children with pervasive developmental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(2), 294–296.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Constantino, J. N., Przybeck, T., Friesen, D., Todd, R. D., & Charles, A. D. F. W. G. P. (2000). Reciprocal social behavior in children with and without pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 21(1), 2–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Constantino, J. N., & Todd, R. D. (2003). Autistic traits in the general population: A twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(5), 524–530.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne, E., & Pierce, K. (2005). Brain overgrowth in autism during a critical time in development: Implications for frontal pyramidal neuron and interneuron development and connectivity. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23(2–3), 153–170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • D’Entremont, B. (2000). A perceptual–attentional explanation of gaze following in 3- and 6-month-olds. Developmental Science, 3(3), 302–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Alexander, A. L., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Gaze-fixation, brain activation, and amygdala volume in unaffected siblings of individuals with autism. Biological Psychiatry, 61(4), 512–520.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Estes, A., Munson, J., Schellenberg, G., Bernier, R., & Abbott, R. (2007). Quantitative assessment of autism symptom-related traits in probands and parents: Broader phenotype autism symptom scale. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(3), 523–536.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deák, G. O., Flom, R. A., & Pick, A. D. (2000). Effects of gesture and target on 12- and 18-month-olds’ joint visual attention to objects in front of or behind them. Developmental Psychology, 36(4), 511–523.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dorris, L., Espie, C. A. E., Knott, F., & Salt, J. (2004). Mind-reading difficulties in the siblings of people with Asperger’s syndrome: Evidence for a genetic influence in the abnormal development of a specific cognitive domain. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(2), 412–418.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Driver, J., Davis, G., Ricciardelli, P., Kidd, P., Maxwell, E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Gaze perception triggers reflexive visuospatial orienting. Visual Cognition, 6(5), 509–540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, C. D. (1990). The Nature and structure of children’s abilities: Evidence from the differential ability scales. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 8(3), 376–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (1998). The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5(3), 490–495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frischen, A., Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Gaze cueing of attention: Visual attention, social cognition, and individual differences. Psychological Bulletin, 133(4), 694–724.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geschwind, D., & Alarcón, M. (2006). Finding genes in spite of heterogeneity: Endophenotypes, and expression profiling in autism. In S. O. Moldin & J. L. R. Rubenstein (Eds.), Understanding autism: From basic neuroscience to treatment (pp. 75–93). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., Briskman, J., & Frith, U. (2001). Exploring the cognitive phenotype of autism: Weak “central coherence” in parents and siblings of children with autism: I. experimental tests. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(3), 299–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heerey, E. A., Keltner, D., & Capps, L. M. (2003). Making sense of self-conscious emotion: Linking theory of mind and emotion in children with autism. Emotion, 3(4), 394–400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Houston-Price, C., Plunkett, K., & Duffy, H. (2006). The use of social and salience cues in early word learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 95(1), 27–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kylliäinen, A., & Hietanen, J. K. (2004). Attention orienting by another’s gaze direction in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(3), 435–444.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule—Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Losh, M., Adolphs, R., Poe, M. D., Couture, S., Penn, D., Baranek, G. T., et al. (2009). Neuropsychological profile of autism and the broad autism phenotype. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(5), 518–526.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loveland, K. A., & Landry, S. H. (1986). Joint attention and language in autism and developmental language delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16(3), 335–349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Merin, N., Young, G., Ozonoff, S., & Rogers, S. (2007). Visual fixation patterns during reciprocal social interaction distinguish a subgroup of 6-month-old infants at-risk for autism from comparison infants. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(1), 108–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2004). 12- and 18-month-old infants follow gaze to spaces behind barriers. Developmental Science, 7(1), F1–F9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C. (2008). The development of gaze following. Child Development Perspectives, 2(2), 66–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(1), 115–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficits of autism: The contributions of nonverbal-communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 27(5), 657–669.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nadig, A. S., Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Rozga, A., Sigman, M., & Rogers, S. J. (2007). A prospective study of response to name in infants at risk for autism. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 161(4), 378–383.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, P. B., Adamson, L. B., & Bakeman, R. (2008). Toddlers’ joint engagement experience facilitates preschoolers’ acquisition of theory of mind. Developmental Science, 11(6), 847–852.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Palmen, S., Pol, H. E. H., Kemner, C., Schnack, H. G., Sitskoorn, M. M., Appels, M. C. M., et al. (2005). Brain anatomy in non-affected parents of autistic probands: A MRI study. Psychological Medicine, 35(10), 1411–1420.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pelphrey, K. A., Shultz, S., Hudac, C. M., & Vander Wyk, B. C. (2011). Research review: Constraining heterogeneity: The social brain and its development in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(6), 631–644.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pelphrey, K. A., Singerman, J. D., Allison, T., & McCarthy, G. (2003). Brain activation evoked by perception of gaze shifts: The influence of context. Neuropsychologia, 41(2), 156–170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interactions in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31(4), 437–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reid, V. M., & Striano, T. (2005). Adult gaze influences infant attention and object processing: Implications for cognitive neuroscience. European Journal of Neuroscience, 21(6), 1763–1766.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ronald, A., Viding, E., Happe, F., & Plomin, R. (2006). Individual differences in theory of mind ability in middle childhood and links with verbal ability and autistic traits: A twin study. Social Neuroscience, 1(3–4), 412–425.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A., Tojo, Y., Dairoku, H., & Hasegawa, T. (2004). Reflexive orienting in response to eye gaze and an arrow in children with and without autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(3), 445–458.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaked, M., Gamliel, I., & Yirmiya, N. (2006). Theory of mind abilities in young siblings of children with autism. Autism, 10(2), 173–187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., Dijamco, A., Gratier, M., & Rozga, A. (2004). Early detection of core deficits in autism. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 10(4), 221–233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., & McGovern, C. W. (2005). Improvement in cognitive and language skills from preschool to adolescence in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(1), 15–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down syndrome, and developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64(1), 114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2008). Modeling longitudinal change in the language abilities of children with autism: Parent behaviors and child characteristics as predictors of change. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1691–1704.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Speer, L. L., Cook, A. E., McMahon, W. M., & Clark, E. (2007). Face processing in children with autism. Autism, 11(3), 265–277.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, S. J., Cantor, R. M., Chung, L., Kim, S., Geschwind, D. H., & Alarcón, M. (2006). Stratification based on language-related endophenotypes in autism: Attempt to replicate reported linkage. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 141B(6), 591–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, M. D., Holt, R. J., Chura, L. R., Suckling, J., Calder, A. J., Bullmore, E. T., et al. (2011). A novel functional brain imaging endophenotype of autism: The neural response to facial expression of emotion. Translational Psychiatry, 1, e19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Striano, T., & Stahl, D. (2005). Sensitivity to triadic attention in early infancy. Developmental Science, 8(4), 333–343.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, M., Finelli, J., Marvin, A., Garrett-Mayer, E., Bauman, M., & Landa, R. (2007). Response to joint attention in toddlers at risk for autism spectrum disorder: A prospective study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(1), 37–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swettenham, J., Condie, S., Campbell, R., Milne, E., & Coleman, M. (2003). Does the perception of moving eyes trigger reflexive visual orienting in autism? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 358(1430), 325–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (1999). A psychological approach to understanding the social and language impairments in autism. International Review of Psychiatry, 11(4), 325–334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. H. G., Waiter, G. D., Perra, O., Perrett, D. I., & Whiten, A. (2005). An fMRI study of joint attention experience. NeuroImage, 25(1), 133–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yoder, P., Stone, W. L., Walden, T., & Malesa, E. (2009). Predicting social impairment and ASD diagnosis in younger siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(10), 1381–1391.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Young, G. S., Merin, N., Rogers, S. J., & Ozonoff, S. (2009). Gaze behavior and affect at 6 months: Predicting clinical outcomes and language development in typically developing infants and infants at risk for autism. Developmental Science, 12(5), 798–814.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the families for participating in this study. We are grateful to the past and present members of the Communication & Play Lab for their continued support. This study was supported by a Grant-In-Aid of Research from the National Academy of Sciences, administered by Sigma Xi The Scientific Research Society, and a Doctoral Student Research Grant from The Graduate Center of City University of New York both awarded to MRS. This research was also supported in part by the RISE Program at Hunter College, GM060665-11. This experiment was carried out by Meghan Swanson as part of her doctoral dissertation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Meghan R. Swanson.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Swanson, M.R., Serlin, G.C. & Siller, M. Broad Autism Phenotype in Typically Developing Children Predicts Performance on an Eye-Tracking Measure of Joint Attention. J Autism Dev Disord 43, 707–718 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1616-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1616-7

Keywords

Navigation