Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 43, Issue 3, pp 695–706 | Cite as

Are the Autism and Positive Schizotypy Spectra Diametrically Opposed in Empathizing and Systemizing?

  • Suzanna N. Russell-SmithEmail author
  • Donna M. Bayliss
  • Murray T. Maybery
  • Rosy L. Tomkinson
Original Paper

Abstract

Crespi and Badcock’s (Behaviour Brain Sci 31: 241–261, 2008) novel theory, which presents autism and positive schizophrenia as diametrical opposites on a cognitive continuum, has received mixed support in the literature to date. The current study aimed to further assess the validity of this theory by investigating predictions in relation to empathizing and systemizing. Specifically, it is predicted by Crespi and Badcock that while mild autistic traits should be associated with a cognitive profile of superior mechanistic cognition (which overlaps with systemizing) but reduced mentalistic cognition (which overlaps with empathizing), positive schizotypy traits should be associated with the opposite profile of superior mentalistic but reduced mechanistic cognition. These predictions were tested in a student sample using a battery of self-report and behavioural measures. The pattern of results obtained provides no support for Crespi and Badcock’s theory.

Keywords

Autism Schizotypy Empathy Systemizing Cognitive profile 

References

  1. Almeida, R., Dickinson, J., Maybery, M., Badcock, J., & Badcock, D. (2010). A new step toward understanding embedded figures test performance in the autism spectrum: The radial frequency search task. Neuropsychologia, 48(2), 374–381.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Austin, E. (2005). Personality correlates of the broader autism phenotype as assessed by the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ). Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 174–183.Google Scholar
  3. Aylward, E., Minshew, N., Goldstein, G., Honeycutt, N., Augustine, A., Yates, K., et al. (1999). MRI volumes of amygdala and hippocampus in non-mentally retarded autistic adolescents and adults. Neurology, 53(9), 2145–2150.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Science, 6, 248–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 163–175.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001a). The “Reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism and normal sex differences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 241–251.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Scahill, V., Lawson, J., & Spong, A. (2001b). Are intuitive physics and intuitive psychology independent? Journal of Developmental and Learning Disorders, 5, 47–78.Google Scholar
  8. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001c). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger Syndrome/high-functioning Autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Baron-Cohen, S., Richler, J., Bisarya, D., Gurunathan, N., & Wheelwright, S. (2003). The systemizing quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 358, 361–374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Batson, C., Eklund, J., Chermok, V., Hoyt, J., & Ortiz, B. (2007). An additional antecedent of empathic concern: Valuing the welfare of the person in need. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 65–74.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Best, C., Moffat, V., Power, M., Owens, D., & Johnstone, E. (2008). The boundaries of the cognitive phenotype of autism: Theory of mind, central coherence and ambiguous figure perception in young people with autistic traits. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 840–847.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Brosnan, M., Ashwin, C., Walker, I., & Donaghue, J. (2010). Can an ‘Extreme Female Brain’ be characterised in terms of psychosis? Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 738–742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Caissie, A., Vigneau, F., & Bors, D. (2009). What does the mental rotation test measure? An analysis of item difficulty and item characteristics. The Open Psychology Journal, 2, 94–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Carroll, J., & Chiew, K. (2006). Sex and discipline: Differences in empathising, systemising and autistic symptomatology: Evidence from a student population. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 949–957.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Cheung, C., Yu, K., Fung, G., Leung, M., Wong, C., Li, Q., et al. (2010). Autistic disorders and schizophrenia: Related or remote? An anatomical likelihood estimation. PLoS One, 5(8), e12233.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cochrane, M., Petch, I., & Pickering, A. (2010). Do measures of schizotypal personality provide non-clinical analogues of schizophrenic symptomatology. Psychiatry Research, 176(2–3), 150–154.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Constantino, J., & Todd, R. (2005). Intergenerational transmission of subthreshold autistic traits in the general population. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 655–660.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Crespi, B., & Badcock, C. (2008). Psychosis and autism as diametrical disorders of the social brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 241–261.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Davis, M. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Del Giudice, M., Angeleri, R., Brizio, A., & Elena, M. (2010). The evolution of autistic-like and schizotypal traits: A sexual selection hypothesis. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 1–18.Google Scholar
  21. Devine, T., Fay, N., & Maybery, M. (2012). The Empathiser-in-Context: Evidence for the Stability and Variability of Empathic Responses for Others in Need of Help. Manuscript in Preparation.Google Scholar
  22. Dziobek, I., Rogers, K., Fleck, S., Bahnemann, M., Heekeren, H., Wolf, O., et al. (2008). Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults with Asperger Syndrome using the Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 464–473.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Focquaert, F., Steven, M., Wolford, G., Colden, A., & Gazzaniga, M. (2007). Empathizing and systemizing cognitive traits in the sciences and humanities. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 619–625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Gooding, D., & Pflum, M. (2011). Theory of Mind and psychometric schizotypy. Psychiatry Research, 188, 217–223.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Grinter, E., Maybery, M., Van Beek, P., Pellicano, E., Badcock, J., & Badcock, D. (2009a). Global visual processing and self-rated autistic-like traits. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1278–1290.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Grinter, E., Van Beek, P., Maybery, M., & Badcock, D. (2009b). Brief report: Visuospatial analysis and self-rated autistic-like traits. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 670–677.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Happé, F. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 129–154.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Henry, J., Bailey, P., & Rendell, P. (2008). Empathy, social functioning and schizotypy. Psychiatry Research, 160, 15–22.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hoekstra, R., Bartels, M., Cath, D., & Boomsma, D. (2008). Factor structure, reliability and criterion validity of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): A study in Dutch population and patient groups. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(8), 1555–1566.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Howard, M., Cowell, P., Boucher, J., Broks, P., Mayes, A., Farrant, A., et al. (2000). Convergent neuroanatomical and behavioural evidence of an amygdala hypothesis of autism. NeuroReport, 11, 2931–2935.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hurst, R., Mitchell, J., Kimbrel, N., Kwapil, T., & Nelson-Gray, R. (2007). Examination of the reliability and factor structure of the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) in a non-clinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 1938–1948.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Jobe, L., & White, S. (2007). Loneliness, social relationships, and a broader autism phenotype in college students. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 1479–1489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Johnson, J. (2005). Dysfunction of the anterior hippocampus: The cause of fundamental schizophrenic symptoms? Medical Hypotheses, 65, 55–60.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kim, J., & Lee, S. (2010). Reliability and validity of the Korean version of the Empathy Quotient Scale. Psychiatry Investigation, 7, 24–30.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Kosaka, H., Omori, M., Murata, T., Iidaka, T., Yamada, H., Okada, T., et al. (2002). Differential amygdala response during facial recognition in patients with schizophrenia: An fMRI study. Schizophrenia Research, 57, 87–95.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Krajmer, P., Janusikova, D., Spajdel, M., & Ostatnikova, D. (2010). Empathizing, systemizing, intuitive physics and folk psychology. Activitas Nervosa Superior Rediviva, 52, 56–61.Google Scholar
  37. Kunihira, Y., Senju, A., Dairoku, H., Wakabayashi, A., & Hasegawa, T. (2006). ‘Autistic’ traits in non-autistic Japanese populations: Relationships with personality traits and cognitive ability. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 533–566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Langdon, R., Coltheart, M., & Ward, P. (2006a). Empathetic perspective-taking is impaired in schizophrenia: Evidence from a study of emotion attribution and theory of mind. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 11, 133–155.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Langdon, R., Corner, T., McLaren, J., Coltheart, M., & Ward, P. (2006b). Attentional orienting triggered by gaze in schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia, 44, 417–429.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Markram, H., Rinaldi, T., & Markram, K. (2007). The Intense World Syndrome – an alternative hypothesis for autism. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 1, 77–96.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Mason, O., Linney, Y., & Claridge, G. (2005). Short scales for measuring schizotypy. Schizophrenia Research, 78, 293–296.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Matsui, M., Sumiyoshi, T., Kato, K., Yoneyama, E., & Kurachi, M. (2004). Neuropsychological profile in patients with schizotypal personality disorder or schizophrenia. Psychological Reports, 94(2), 387–397.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Maurage, P., Grynberg, D., Noel, X., Joassin, F., Phillippot, P., Hanak, C., et al. (2011). Dissociation between cognitive and affective empathy in alcoholism: A specific deficit for the emotional dimension. Alcholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 35(9), 1–7.Google Scholar
  44. Mayer, J., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence. In P. Salovey & D. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional development, emotional literacy, and emotional intelligence (pp. 3–34). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  45. Muncer, S., & Ling, J. (2006). Psychometric analysis of the Empathy Quotient (EQ) Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 1111–1119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Nettle, D. (2006). Schizotypy and mental health among poets, visual artists, and mathematicians. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 876–890.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Nezlek, J., Feist, G., Wilson, C., & Plesko, R. (2001). Day-to-day variability in empathy as a function of daily events and mood. Journal of Research in Personality, 35, 401–423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Ozer, D. (1987). Personality, intelligence, and spatial visualization: Correlates of mental rotations test performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 129–134.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B., & Rogers, S. (1990). Are there emotion perception deficits in young autistic children? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31(3), 343–361.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Peters, M., Laeng, B., Latham, K., Jackson, M., Zaiyouna, R., & Richardson, C. (1995). A redrawn Vandenberg and Kuse mental rotations test: Different versions and factors that affect performance. Brain and Cognition, 28, 39–58.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Pierce, K., Muller, R., Ambrose, J., Allen, G., & Courchesne, E. (2001). Face processing occurs outside the fusiform ‘face area’ in autism: Evidence from functional MRI. Brain, 124, 2059–2073.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Rogers, K., Dziobek, I., Hassenstab, J., Wolf, O., & Convit, A. (2007). Who cares? revisiting empathy in Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 709–715.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Russell-Smith, S., Maybery, M., & Bayliss, D. (2010). Are the autism and positive schizotypy spectra diametrically opposed in local versus global processing? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(8), 968–977.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Russell-Smith, S., Maybery, M., & Bayliss, D. (2011). Relationships between autistic-like and schizotypy traits: An analysis using the Autism Spectrum Quotient and Oxford-Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(2), 128–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Russell-Smith, S., Maybery, M., Bayliss, D., & Sng, A. (in press). Support for a link between the local processing bias and social deficits in autism: An investigation of Embedded Figures Test performance in non-clinical individuals. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1506-z.
  56. Schumann, C., Hamstra, J., Goodlin-Jones, B., Lotspeich, L., Kwon, H., Buonocore, M., et al. (2004). The amygdala is enlarged in children but not adolescents with autism; the hippocampus is enlarged at all ages. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 6392–6401.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Soulières, I., Zeffiro, T., Girard, M., & Mottron, L. (2011). Enhanced mental image mapping in autism. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 848–857.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Sparks, B., Friedman, S., Shaw, D., Aylward, E., Echelard, D., Artru, A., et al. (2002). Brain structural abnormalities in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Neurology, 59, 184–192.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Stewart, M., & Austin, E. (2009). The Structure of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from a student sample in Scotland. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 224–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Valla, J., Ganzel, B., Yoder, K., Chen, G., Lyman, L., Sidari, A., et al. (2010). More than maths and mindreading: Sex differences in empathizing/systemizing covariance. Autism Research, 3, 174–184.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Van Lange, P. (2008). Does empathy trigger only altruistic motivation? how about selflessness of justice? Emotion, 8(6), 766–774.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Velakoulis, D., Wood, S., Wong, M., McGorry, P., Yung, A., Phillips, L., et al. (2006). Hippocampal and amygdala volumes according to psychosis stage and diagnosis: A magnetic resonance imaging study of chronic schizophrenia, first-episode psychosis, and ultra-high-risk individuals. Archive of General Psychiatry, 63(2), 139–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Voracek, M., & Dressler, S. (2006). Lack of correlation between digit ratio (2D:4D) and Baron-Cohen’s “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test, empathy, systemising, and autism-spectrum quotients in a general population sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 1481–1491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Wakabayashi, A., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Goldenfeld, N., Delaney, J., Fine, D., et al. (2006). Development of short forms of the Empathy Quotient (EQ-Short) and the Systemizing Quotient (SQ-Short). Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 929–940.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Wakabayashi, A., Baron-Cohen, S., Uchiyama, T., Yoshida, Y., Kuroda, M., & Wheelwright, S. (2007). Empathizing and systemizing in adults with and without Autism Spectrum Conditions: Cross-cultural stability. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1823–1832.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Walter, E., Dassonville, P., & Bochsler, T. (2009). A specific trait that modulates visuospatial illusion susceptibility. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(2), 339–349.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Suzanna N. Russell-Smith
    • 1
    Email author
  • Donna M. Bayliss
    • 1
  • Murray T. Maybery
    • 1
  • Rosy L. Tomkinson
    • 1
  1. 1.Neurocognitive Development Unit, School of PsychologyUniversity of Western AustraliaCrawley, PerthAustralia

Personalised recommendations