Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 43, Issue 3, pp 616–628 | Cite as

Emotional Resonance Deficits in Autistic Children

  • Alessandro GrecucciEmail author
  • Paolo Brambilla
  • Roma Siugzdaite
  • Danielle Londero
  • Franco Fabbro
  • Raffaella Ida Rumiati
Original Paper

Abstract

According to some theories imitation, defined as an action resonance mechanism, is deficient in autism. In contrast, other theories (e.g., the “top down control of imitation” hypothesis) state that the problem is not in imitation per se but in the way social cues modulate imitative responses. In this study, 15 high-functioning children with autism and 15 matched controls were tested for their ability to imitate finger movements preceded by neutral and emotional facial expressions (primes) in a stimulus–response compatibility task. Hand movements performed after neutral expressions did not differ between the two groups (i.e., they both showed a normal imitative tendency). However, hand movements performed after emotional expressions significantly differed between the two populations, with controls, but not autistic spectrum disorder (ASD), showing enhanced imitation in the emotional condition. This study supports the view that, in ASD, imitation abilities are spared but they are not modulated according to the emotional and social context.

Keywords

Imitation Action resonance Empathy Emotions Mirror neurons 

Notes

Acknowledgments

PB was partly supported by the American Psychiatric Institute for Research and Education (APIRE), by the Italian Ministry for University and Research and by the Italian Ministry of Health (IRCCS “E. Medea”). RIR was supported by a grant awarded by Beneficentia.

References

  1. Aldridge, M. A., Stone, K. R., Sweeney, M. H., & Bower, T. G. R. (2000). Preverbal children with autism understand the intentions of others. Developmental Science, 3, 294–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Ashwin, C., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., O’Riordan, M., & Bullmore, E. T. (2007). Differential activation of the amygdala and the ‘social brain’ during fearful face-processing in Asperger Syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2–14.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Avikainen, S., Wohlschlager, A., Liuhanen, S., Hanninen, R., & Hari, R. (2003). Impaired mirror-image imitation in Asperger and high-functioning autistic subjects. Current Biology, 13, 339–341.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baron-Cohen, S. (1993). From attention-goal psychology to beliefdesire psychology: The development of a theory of mind and its dysfunction. In S. Baron-Cohen (Ed.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Bullmore, E. T., Wheelwright, S., Ashwin, C., Williams, S. C. (2000). The amygdala theory of autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Review, 24(3), 355–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, T., & Cohen, D. (1993). Understanding other minds: perspectives from autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Bastiaansen, J. A. C. J., Thioux, M., & Keysers, C. (2009). Evidence for mirror system in emotion. Philosophical Transaction of the Royal Society, 364, 2391–2404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Beadle-Brown, J., & Whiten, A. (2004). Elicited imitation in children and adults with autism: Is there a deficit? Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 29(2), 141–157.Google Scholar
  9. Bird, G., Catmur, C., Silani, G., Frith, C., & Frith, U. (2006). Attention does not modulate neural responses to social stimuli in autism spectrum disorders. Neuroimage, 31(4), 1614–1624.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bird, G., Leighton, J., Press, C., & Heyes, C. (2007). Intact automatic imitation of human and robot actions in autism spectrum disorders. Proceedings of Biological Science, 274(1628), 3027–3031.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Brass, M., Bekkering, H., & Prinz, W. (2001). Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychologia, 106, 3–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Brass, M., Derfuss, J., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2005). The inhibition of imitative and overlearned responses: A functional double dissociation. Neuropsychologia, 43(1), 89–98.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2001). Understanding of others’ intentions in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 589–599.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Catmur, C., & Heyes, C. (2011). Time course analyses confirm independence of imitative and spatial compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(2), 409–421.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Celani, G., Battacchi, M. W., & Arcidiacono, L. (1999). The understanding of the emotional meaning of facial expressions in people with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 57–66.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cook, J. L., & Bird, G. (2011). A typical social modulation of imitation in autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(6), 1045–1051.Google Scholar
  17. Corden, B., Critchley, H. D., Skuse, D., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Fear recognition ability predicts differences in social cognitive and neural functioning in men. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 889–897.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Crescentini, C., Mengotti, P., Grecucci, A., Rumiati, R. I. (2011). The effect of observed biological and non biological movements on action imitation: An fMRI study. Brain Research, 1420, 80–92.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., et al. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 28–30.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 479–485.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Dawson, G., Webb, S., Schellenberg, G. D., Dager, S., Friedman, S., Aylward, E., et al. (2002). Defining the broader phenotype of autism: Genetic, brain and behavioral perspectives. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 581–611.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Decéty, J., & Chaminade, T. (2003). Neural correlates of feeling sympathy. Neuropsychologia. Special Issue on Social Cognition, 41, 127–138.Google Scholar
  23. Dimberg, U. (1982). Facial reactions to facial expressions. Psychophysiology, 19, 643–647.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., & Elmehed, K. (2000). Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions. Psychological Science, 11, 86–89.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1978). Facial action coding system: A technique for the measurement of facial movement. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
  26. Frith, C., & Frith, U. (2006). The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron, 50(4), 531–534.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Grecucci, A., Balaban, E., Buiatti, T., Budai, R., & Rumiati, R. I. (2009). The emotional control of action: ERP evidence. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 147, 37–49.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Grecucci, A., Cooper, R., & Rumiati, R. I. (2007). A computational model of action resonance and its modulation by emotional stimulation. Cognitive Systems Research, 8(3), 143–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Grecucci, A., Koch, I., & Rumiati, R. I. (2011). The role of emotional context in facilitating imitative actions. Acta Psychologica, 138(2), 311–315.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hadjikhani, N., Joseph, R. M., Manoach, D. S., Naik, P., Snyder, J., Dominick, K., et al. (2009). Body expressions of emotion do not trigger fear contagion in autism spectrum disorder. Social affective and cognitive neuroscience, 4(1), 70–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hamilton, A. F. (2008). Emulation and mimicry for social interaction: A theoretical approach to imitation in autism. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(1),101–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hamilton, A. F., Brindley, R. M., & Frith, U. (2007). Imitation and action understanding in autistic spectrum disorders: How valid is the hypothesis of a deficit in the mirror neuron system? Neuropsychologia, 45, 1859–1868.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988). What’s in a face? The case of autism. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 441–453.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1989). Naming emotions in faces and voices. Abilities and Disabilities in Autism and Mental Retardation, 1, 237–250.Google Scholar
  35. Hubert, B. E., Wicker, B., Monfardini, E., & Deruelle, C. (2009). Electrodermal reactivity to emotion processing in adults with autistic spectrum disorders. Autism, 13(1), 9–19.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Humphreys, K., Minshew, N., Leonard, G. L., & Behrmann, M. (2007). A fine grained analysis of facial expression processing in high-functioning adults with autism. Neuropsychologia, 45, 685–695.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Iacoboni, M. (2009). The neurobiology of imitation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 19(6), 661–665.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Iacoboni, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 942–951.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger’s syndrome: Is global coherence impaired? Psychological Medicine, 30, 1169–1187.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Lacroix, A., Guidetti, M., Rogé, B., & Reilly, J. (2009). Recognition of emotional and nonemotional facial expressions: A comparison between Williams syndrome and autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30(5), 976–985.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Lakin, J., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14, 334–339.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Leighton, J., Bird, G., Charman, T., & Heyes, C. (2008). Weak imitative performance is not due to a functional ‘mirroring’ deficit in adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Neuropsychologia, 7, 1041–1049.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Loveland, K. A., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Chen, Y. R., Ortegon, J., Pearson, D. A., Brelsford, K.A., et al. (1997). Emotion recognition in autism: Verbal and nonverbal information. Developmental Psychopathology, 9(3), 579–593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Loveland, K. A., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Pearson, D. A., Brelsford, K. A., Ortegon, J., & Chen, R. (1995). Imitation and expression of facial affect in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6(3), 433–444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Lundquist, L. O., & Dimberg, A. (1995). Facial expressions are contagious. Journal of Psychophysiology, 9, 203–211.Google Scholar
  46. McIntosh, D. N. (1996). Facial feedback hypotheses: Evidence, implications, and directions. Motivation and Emotion, 20, 121–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. McIntosh, D. N., Druckman, D., & Zajonc, R. B. (1994). Socially-induced affect. In D. Druckman & R. A. Bjork (Eds.), Learning, remembering, believing: Enhancing human performance (pp. 251–276). Washington, DC: National Academy Press. (364–371).Google Scholar
  48. McIntosh, D. N., Reichmann-Decker, A., Winkielman, P., & Wilbarger, J. L. (2006). When the social mirror breaks: Deficits in automatic, but not voluntary, mimicry of emotional facial expressions in autism. Developmental Science, 9, 295–302.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Moody, E. J., & McIntosh, D. N. (2006). Mimicry and autism: Bases and consequences of rapid, automatic matching behavior. In S.J. Rogers, J. Williams (Eds.), Imitation and the social mind: Autism and typical development. New York: Guilford Press (in press).Google Scholar
  50. Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184–211.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Pierno, A. C., Mari, M., Lusher, D., & Castiello, U. (2008). Robotic movements elecits visuomotor priming in children with autism. Neuropshychologia, 46(2), 448–454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Ponnet, K. S., Roeyers, H., Buysse, A., De Clercq, A., & Van der Heyden, E. (2004). Advanced mind-reading in adults with Asperger syndrome. Autism, 8(3), 249–266.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Press, C., Richardson, D., & Bird, G. (2010). Intact imitation of emotional facial actions in autism spectrum conditions. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3291–3297.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Ramachandran, V. S., & Oberman, L. M. (2006). Evidence for deficits in mirror neuron function, multisensory integration, and soundform symbolism in autism spectrum disorders. Poster presented at the 13th annual meeting of the cognitive neuroscience society, New York.Google Scholar
  55. Rieffe, C., Meerum Terwogt, M., & Stockmann, L. (2000). Understanding atypical emotions among children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 195–203.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Rizzolatti, G., & Fabbri-Destro, M. (2010). Mirror neurons: From discovery to autism. Experimental Brain Research, 200(3–4), 223–237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T., & Wehner, E. (2003). Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 763–781.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Rumiati, R. I., & Tessari, A. (2007). Automatic and strategic effects in human imitation. In P.H, Kawato, & I. Rossetti (Eds.), Sensorimotor foundations of human cognition: Attention and performance, Vol. XXII. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  59. Schumann, C. M., Bauman, M. D., & Amaral, D. G. (2011). Abnormal structure or function of the amygdala is a common component of neurodevelopmental disorders. Neuropsychologia, 49(4), 745–759.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Sparks, B. F., Friedman, S. D., Shaw, D. W., Aylward, E. H., Echelard, D., et al. (2002). Brain structural abnormalities in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Neurology, 59, 184–192.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Spengler, S., Bird, G., & Brass, M. (2010). Hyperimitation of actions is related to reduced understanding of others’ minds in autism spectrum conditions. Biological Psychiatry, 68(12), 1148–1155.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J., Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T. A., et al. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168, 242–249.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Welchew, D. E., Ashwin, C., Berkouk, K., Salvador, R., Suckling, J., Baron-Cohen, S., et al. (2005). Functional disconnectivity of the medial temporal lobe in Asperger’s syndrome. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 991–998.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., & Singh, T. (2004). Systematic review of action imitation in autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 285–299.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews, 25, 287–295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Wright, B., Clarke, N., Jordan, J., Young, A. W., Clarke, P., Miles, J., et al. (2008). Emotion recognition in faces and the use of visual context in young people with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 12(6), 607–626.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Alessandro Grecucci
    • 1
    • 2
    Email author
  • Paolo Brambilla
    • 3
    • 5
  • Roma Siugzdaite
    • 4
  • Danielle Londero
    • 3
  • Franco Fabbro
    • 3
    • 6
  • Raffaella Ida Rumiati
    • 1
  1. 1.Cognitive Neuroscience SectorInternational School for Advanced Study (SISSA/ISAS)TriesteItaly
  2. 2.Department of Cognitive Science and Education (DiSCoF)/Center for Mind/Brain Sciences (CIMeC), Faculty of Cognitive ScienceUniversity of TrentoTrentoItaly
  3. 3.Scientific Institute, IRCCS “E. Medea”UdineItaly
  4. 4.Medical Image and Signal Processing (MEDISIP), Department of Electronics and Information SystemsGhent University Hospital IBiTechGhentBelgium
  5. 5.DISM, Inter-University Centre for Behavioural Neurosciences (ICBN)University of UdineUdineItaly
  6. 6.Department of Human SciencesUniversity of UdineUdineItaly

Personalised recommendations