Moral and Social Reasoning in Autism Spectrum Disorders

Abstract

This study compared moral and social reasoning in individuals with and without autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Ten familiar schoolyard transgressions were shown to 18 participants with and 18 participants without ASD. They judged the appropriateness of the behavior and explained their judgments. Analysis of the rationales revealed that participants with typical development used significantly more abstract rules than participants with ASD, who provided more nonspecific condemnations of the behaviors. Both groups judged social conventional transgressions to be more context-bound than moral transgressions, with this distinction more pronounced in typically developing individuals, who also provided significantly more examples of situations in which the depicted behaviors would be acceptable. The educational implications of these findings for individuals with ASD are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

Fig. 1

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV), 4th edn. Washington DC: APA.

  2. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (fourth edition-revised). Washington DC: APA.

  3. Anderson, L. M. (1989). Implementing instructional programs to promote meaningful, self-regulated meaning. In J. E. Brophy (Ed.), Advances in research technology (Vol. 1, pp. 311–345). NYC: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Best, C. A., Minshew, N. J., & Strauss, M. S. (2010). Gender discrimination of eyes and mouths by individuals with autism. Autism Research, 3(1), 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Billings, D. (2007). Teaching for transfer of core/key skills in higher education: Cognitive skills. Higher Education, 53, 483–516.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2002). Exploring the borderlands of autistic disorder and specific language impairment: A study using standardized diagnostic instruments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(7), 917–929.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Blair, R. J. R. (1996). Brief report: Morality in the autistic child. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26, 571–579.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Brown, J. S., Collins, A., & Duguid, P. (1991). Situated cognition and the culture of learning. In M. Yazdani & R. W. Lawler (Eds.), Artificial intelligence and education (Vol. 2, pp. 245–268). Westport, CT, US: Ablex Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Corbett, B. A., Constantine, L. J., Hendren, R., Rocke, D., & Ozonoff, S. J. (2009). Examining executive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorders, attention deficit hyperactivity disorders, and typical development. Psychiatry Research, 166, 210–222.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Crane-Ross, D., & Tisak, M. S. (1995). Mixed-domain events: The influence of moral and conventional components on the development of social reasoning. Early Education and Development, 6, 347–365.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Dawson, G., Osterling, J., Meltzoff, A., & Kuhl, P. (2000). Case study of the development of an infant with autism from birth to two years of age. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21(3), 299–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Forbes, C. E., & Grafman, J. (2010). The role of the human prefrontal cortex in social cognition and moral judgment. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 33, 299–324.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Frith, U. (1997). The neurocognitive basis of autism. Trends in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 73–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Geurts, H. M., Corbett, B., & Solomon, M. (2009). The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism. Trends in Cognitive Science, 13(2), 74–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Grant, C., Boucher, J., Riggs, K., & Grayson, A. (2005). Moral understanding in children with autism. Autism, 9, 317–331.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Hahn, U., & Chater, N. (1998). Similarity and rules: Distinct? Exhaustive? Empirically distinguishable? Cognition, 65(2–3), 197–230.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak central coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5–25.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Harvey, R. J., Fletcher, J., & French, D. J. (2001). Social reasoning: A source of influence on social aggression. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 447–469.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Hill, E. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 26–32.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Humphries, M. L., Parker, B. L., & Jagers, R. J. (2000). Predictors of moral reasoning among African American children: A preliminary study. Journal of Black Psychology, 26, 51–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Killen, M. (1991). Social and moral development in early childhood. In W. M. Kurtines & J. L. Gewirtz (Eds.), Handbook of moral behavior and development (pp. 115–138). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Elbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Killen, M., Lee-Kim, J., McGlothlin, H., & Stanger, C. (2006). How children and adolescents evaluate gender and racial exclusion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67(4, serial no. 271).

    Google Scholar 

  23. Killen, M., Margie, N. G., & Sinno, S. (2006b). Morality in the context of intergroup relationships. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 155–183). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Killen, M., & Smetana, J. G. (1999). Social interactions in preschool classrooms and the development of young children’s conceptions of the personal. Child Development, 70(2), 486–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Killen, M., & Smetana, J. G. (2008). Moral judgment and moral neuroscience: Intersections, definitions and issues. Child Development Perspectives, 2(1), 1–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Klinger, L. G., & Dawson, G. (1991). A fresh look at categorization skills in persons with autism. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.), Learning and cognition in autism (pp. 119–136). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Kohlberg, L. (1984). The psychology of moral development: The nature and validity of moral stages. San Francisco: Harper Row.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Kohlberg, L., & Kramer, R. (1969). Continuities and discontinuities in childhood and adult moral development. Human Development, 12, 93–120.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Leslie, A., Malton, R., & DiCorcia, J. A. (2006). Transgressor, victims and cry babies: Is basic moral judgment spare din autism? Social Neuroscience, 1, 270–283.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule–generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview—revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Loth, E., Gomez, J. C., & Happe, F. (2008). Event schemas in autism spectrum disorders: The role of theory of mind and weak central coherence. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 449–463.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Loveland, K. A., Pearson, D. A., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Ortegon, J., & Gibbs, M. (2001). Judgments of social appropriateness by children and adolescents with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(4), 367–376.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Meiran, N. (2010). Task-switching: Mechanisms underlying rigid vs. flexible self-control. In R. R. Hassin, K. N. Ochsner, & Y. Trope (Eds.), Self-control in society, mind and Brain (pp. 202–220). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7(3), 134–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Newell, B. R., & Broder, A. (2008). Cognitive processes, models and metaphors in decision making. Judgment and Decision Making, 3(3), 195–204.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Nucci, L. P., & Turiel, E. (1978). Social interactions and the development of social concepts in preschool children. Child Development, 49, 400–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Perkins, D. N., & Salomon, G. (1987). Transfer and teaching thinking. In D. N. Perkins, J. Lockhead, & J. Bishop (Eds.), Thinking: The second international conference (pp. 285–303). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Elbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Porter, M., & Coltheart, M. (2006). Global and local processing in Williams syndrome, autism and Down syndrome: Perception, attention and construction. Developmental Neuropsychology, 30(3), 771–789.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Shantz, C. U. (1999). Commentary: Development, modalities, and relationships. In W. A. Collins & B. Laursen (Eds.), Relationships as developmental contexts, The Minnesota symposia on child psychology (Vol. 30, pp. 315–322). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Shulman, C., Yirmiya, N., & Greenbaum, C. W. (1995). From categorization to classification: A comparison among individuals with autism, mental retardation and normal development. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104(4), 601–609.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Smetana, J. (1981). Preschool children’s conception of moral and social rules. Child Development, 52(4), 1333–1336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Smetana, J. G. (1989). Toddlers’ social interactions in the context of moral and conventional transgressions in the home. Developmental Psychology, 25(4), 499–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Smetana, J. (2002). Culture, autonomy, and personal jurisdiction in adolescent-parent relationships. In R. V. Kail & H. W. Reese (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 29, pp. 51–87). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Smetana, J. (2006). Social-cognitive domain theory: Consistencies and variations in children’s moral and social judgments. In M. Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 119–153). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Smetana, J. G., & Braeges, J. L. (1990). The development of toddler’s moral and conventional judgments. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly: Journal of Developmental Psychology, 36(3), 329–346.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Solomon, M., Ozonoff, S., Cummings, N., & Carter, C. S. (2008). Cognitive control in autism spectrum disorders. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 26, 239–247.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Song, M., Smetana, J. G., & Kim, S. (1987). Korean children’s conceptions of moral and conventional transgressions. Developmental Psychology, 23, 577–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Sousa, P., Holbrook, C., & Piazza, J. (2009). The morality of harm. Cognition, 113(1), 80–92.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1995). Relevance: Communication and cognition (2nd ed.). Malden: Blackwell Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Stich, S., Fessler, D. M. T., & Kelly, D. (2009). On morality of harm: A response to Sousa, Holbrook and Piazza. Cognition, 113(1), 93–97.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Surian, L., Baron-Cohen, S., & van der Lely, H. (1996). Are children with autism deaf to Grecian maxims? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 1(1), 55–72.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (1995). Attributing mental states to story characters: A comparison of narratives produced by autistic and mentally retarded individuals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16(3), 241–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Takeda, T., Kasai, K., & Kato, N. (2007). Moral judgment in high-functioning pervasive developmental disorders. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 61, 407–414.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Teunisse, J. P., Cools, A. R., van Spaendonck, K. P. M., Aerts, F. H. T. M., & Berger, H. J. C. (2001). Cognitive styles in high-functioning adolescents with autistic disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 55–66.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Tisak, M. S., Tisak, J., & Goldstein, S. E. (2006). Aggression, delinquency, and morality: A social-cognitive perspective. In M. Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 611–632). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Tisak, M. S., & Turiel, E. (1988). Variation in seriousness of transgressions and children’s moral and conventional concepts. Developmental Psychology, 24, 352–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge: Morality and convention. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Turiel, E. (1989). Domain-specific social judgments and domain ambiguities. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 35, 89–114.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Turiel, E. (2006). The development of morality. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Volume 3: Social, emotional and personality development (6th ed., pp. 789–857). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Turiel, E. (2008). Thoughts about actions in social domains: Morality, social conventions and social interactions. Cognitive Development, 23, 136–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Turiel, E., & Wainryb, C. (1998). Concepts of freedoms and rights in a traditional, hierarchically organized society. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16(3), 375–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Volkmar, F. R., State, M., & Klin, A. (2009). Autism and autism spectrum disorder: Diagnostic issues for the coming decade. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(1–2), 108–115.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Wainryb, C. (2006). Moral development in culture: Diversity, tolerance and justice. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 211–240). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Wechsler, D. (1991). WISC-III: Wechsler intelligence scale for children. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  66. World Health Organization. (1992). International classification of diseases, 10th edn. Geneva: WHO.

  67. Zelazo, P. D., & Frye, D. (1998). Cognitive complexity and control: The development of executive function in childhood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7(4), 121–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Cory Shulman.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Shulman, C., Guberman, A., Shiling, N. et al. Moral and Social Reasoning in Autism Spectrum Disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 42, 1364–1376 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1369-8

Download citation

Keywords

  • Autism spectrum disorders
  • Social and moral reasoning
  • Abstract thinking