Abstract
The impact of bilingual exposure on language learning has not been systematically studied in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. This study compared the social abilities and language levels of children (mean age = 56 months) with ASDs from bilingual (n = 45) and monolingual (n = 30) environments. Bilingually-exposed children were subgrouped based on simultaneous bilingual exposure from infancy (SIM, n = 24) versus sequential post-infancy bilingual exposure (SEQ, n = 21). Despite significantly different amounts of bilingual exposure across all groups (p = <0.001) and significantly stronger social interaction scores in the SIM group compared to the SEQ group on the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales-II Interpersonal subdomain (p = 0.025), there were no significant group differences in language level. Bilingually-exposed children with ASDs did not experience additional delays in language development.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.
References
Adamson, L., Bakeman, R., Deckner, D., & Romski, M. (2009). Joint engagement and the emergence of language in children with autism and Down Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 84–96.
Bialystok, E. (2001). Bilingualism in development. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Caselli, M., & Casadio, P. (1995). Il primo vocabulario del bambino: Guida all’uso del questionario MacArthur per la valutazione della comunicazione e del linguaggio nei primi anni di vita. Milan: FrancoAngeli.
Constantino, J. N. (2002). The social responsiveness scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
Constantino, J., & Gruber, C. (2005). Manual: Social responsiveness scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
Dale, P. S., Fenson, L., & Thal, D. (1993). Some suggestions for the adaptation of the MacArthur communicative development inventories to additional languages (unpublished manuscript).
Feltmate, K., & Kay-Raining Bird, E. (2008). Language learning in four bilingual children with Down Syndrome: A detailed analysis of vocabulary and morphosyntax. Canadian Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, 32, 6–20.
Fennell, C., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. (2007). Using speech sounds to guide word learning: The case of bilingual infants. Child Development, 78, 1510–1525.
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., et al. (1993). MacArthur communicative development inventories: User’s guide and technical manual. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.
Frank, I., Poulin-Dubois, D., & Trudeau, N. (1997). Inventaires MacArthur du développement de la communication: Mots et énoncés. Montreal: Concordia University.
Geangu, E., & Benga, O. (2006). Chestionarul MacArthur de Evaluare a Dezvoltarii Abilitatilor de Comunicare: Cuvinte si Propozitii. Cluj-Napoca, Romania: Babes-Bolyai University (unpublished manuscript).
Genesee, F. (2006). Bilingual first language acquisition in perspective. In P. McCurdle & E. Hoff (Eds.), Childhood bilingualism: Research on infancy through school age (pp. 45–67). Tonawanda, NY: Multilingual Matters, Ltd.
Gervain, J., & Werker, J. (2008). How infant speech perception contributes to language acquisition. Language and Linguistics Compass, 2, 1149–1170.
Gutierrez-Clellen, V. F., Simon-Cereijido, G., & Wagner, C. (2008). Bilingual children with language impairment: A comparison with monolinguals and second language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 3–19.
Hambly, C., & Fombonne, E. (2009, May). The impact of bilingual exposure on the expressive language of children with autism spectrum disorders. Poster session presented at the annual meeting of the international society for autism research, Chicago, IL.
Jackson-Maldonado, D., Thal, D., Marchman, V., Newton, T., Fenson, L., & Conboy, B. (2003). MacArthur Inventarios del Desarrollo de Habilidades Comunicativas. User’s guide and technical manual. Baltimore: Brookes.
Kay-Raining Bird, E., Cleave, P., Trudeau, N., Thordardottir, E., Sutton, A., & Thorpe, A. (2005). The language abilities of bilingual children with Down Syndrome. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 14, 187–199.
Kazemi, Y., Nematzadeh, S., Hajian, T., Heidari, M., Daneshpajouh, T., & Mirmoeini, M. (2008). Finding the validity and reliability coefficient of McArthur-Bates communicative development inventory in Farsi. Journal of Research in Rehabilitation Sciences, 3, 45–51.
Kovács, A., & Mehler, J. (2009). Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the United States of America, 106, 6556–6560.
Kremer-Sadlik, T. (2005). To be or not to be bilingual: Autistic children from multilingual families. In J. Cohen, K. McAlister, K. Rolstad, & J. MacSwan (Eds.), Proceedings of the 4th international symposium on bilingualism (pp. 1225–1234). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Kuhl, P., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., & Dawson, G. (2005). Links between social and linguistic processing of speech in preschool children with autism: Behavioral and electrophysiological measures. Developmental Science, 8, F1–F12.
Le Couteur, A., Lord, C., & Rutter, M. (2003). The autism diagnostic interview—revised (ADI-R). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Leadbitter, K., Hudry, K., & the Preschool Autism Communication Trial consortium. (2009, May). Does bilingualism affect language development in autism? Poster session presented at the international meeting for autism research, Chicago, IL.
Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B., DiLavore, P., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–233.
Maital, S., Dromi, E., Sagi, A., & Bornstein, M. H. (2000). The hebrew communicative development inventory: Language specific properties and cross-linguistic generalizations. Journal of Child Language, 27, 1–25.
McLaughlin, B. (1978). Second language acquisition in childhood. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Paradis, J., Crago, M., Genesee, F., & Rice, M. (2003). Bilingual children with specific language impairment: How do they compare with their monolingual peers? Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 46, 113–127.
Parish-Morris, J., Hennon, E., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2007). Children with autism illuminate the role of social intention in word learning. Child Development, 78, 1265–1287.
Patterson, J. (2000). Observed and reported expressive vocabulary and word combinations in bilingual toddlers: Research note. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 43, 121–128.
Pearson, B., Fernandez, S., & Oller, D. (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: Comparisons to monolingual norms. Language Learning, 43, 93–120.
Petersen, J., Marinova-Todd, S., & Mirenda, P. (2011, June 29). Lexical skills in bilingual children with autism Spectrum Disorder: An exploratory study (manuscript submitted for publication) (personal communication).
Pons, F., Lewkowicz, D., Soto-Faraco, S., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2009). Narrowing of intersensory speech perception in infancy. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the United States of America, 106, 10598–10602.
Russo, N., Nicol, T., Trommer, B., Zecker, S., & Kraus, N. (2009). Brainstem transcription of speech is disrupted in children with autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Science, 12, 557–567.
Sebastián-Gallés, N., & Bosch, L. (2009). Developmental shift in the discrimination of vowel contrasts in bilingual infants: Is the distributional account all there is to it? Developmental Science, 12, 874–887.
Sethuraman, N. (2008). Language development in Tamil. (manuscript in preparation).
Seung, H., Siddiq, S., & Elder, J. (2006). Intervention outcomes of a bilingual child with autism. Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology, 14, 53–63.
Sparrow, S., Cicchetti, D., & Balla, D. (2005). Vineland adaptive behavior scales (2nd ed.). Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessment.
Sundara, M., Polka, L., & Molnar, M. (2008). Development of coronal stop perception: Bilingual infants keep pace with their monolingual peers. Cognition, 108, 232–242.
Sundara, M., & Scutellaro, A. (2010). Rhythmic distance between languages affects the development of speech perception in bilingual infants. Journal of Phonetics. doi:10.1016/j.wocn.2010.08.2006.
Weikum, W., Vouloumanos, A., Navarra, J., Soto-Faraco, S., Sebastián-Gallés, N., & Werker, J. (2007). Visual language discrimination in infancy. Science, 316, 1159.
Werker, J., & Byers-Heinlein, K. (2008). Bilingualism in infancy: First steps in perception and comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 144–151.
Werker, J., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Fennell, C. (2009). Bilingual beginnings to learning words. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences, 364, 3649–3663.
Werker, J., & Tees, R. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 49–63.
Wu, J. (1997). Language, play and general development for Chinese infant-toddlers: Using adapted assessments. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Colorado, Boulder.
Yu, B. (2007, November). Language socialization of an autistic child in a bilingual home. Poster presented at the American speech-language hearing association annual convention, Boston, MA.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by a grant to both authors from Autism Speaks.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Appendices
Appendix 1: Language Environment Interview: Script and Scoring for Language Exposure Questions
The following information was collected for every adult caregiver who regularly provided more than 5 h per week of childcare in the home: (a) place of birth, (b) language(s) spoken at home, (c) fluency in each language spoken at home, (d) languages spoken to the child with an ASD, and (e) reasons for any changes in language(s) spoken to the child. For the fluency assessment (question c), caregivers were categorized as ‘native-level’ speakers only if they reported mastery of the language such that they make no pronunciation, grammar, or vocabulary errors and would be recognized as a native speaker of the language by other native speakers. An estimate of language exposure (e.g., % French vs. English input) was recorded for each 6 month period. Finally, the amount of time in childcare was recorded for each 6 month period: researchers coded parent childcare hours using the following 3 levels based on parental employment outside the home: parent not employed or ‘homemaker’, parent employed 5–15 h per week, or parent employed >15 h per week. For non-parent caregivers, hours spent providing childcare were categorized as 5–15 h per week versus >15 h per week.
The following information was collected for childcare in a group setting or intensive therapy program where multiple individuals provided care for the child: (a) languages spoken to the general group of children, (b) languages spoken directly to the child with an ASD, (c) caregivers’ level of fluency in each language(s) used in the environment (e.g., “Were all the daycare workers fluent in French?”), and (d) reasons for any changes in language(s) spoken to the child. For each 6 month period of care, language exposure estimates were elicited (e.g., % French vs. English input) as well as the amount of time the child spent in that setting (e.g., 5–15 h per week vs. >15 h per week).
Following the interview, data from each caregiver were transferred onto a summary score chart for each 6-month period. An algorithm weighted the exposure estimate data based on how much time each caregiver likely spent with the child in direct one-to-one interaction during each 6 month period. For example, exposure from parents with primary childcare responsibilities (e.g., ‘homemakers’) received the highest weight (9 points) whereas exposure through part-time care in a group daycare setting received the lowest weight (1 point). Points assigned to caregivers who provided bilingual input were divided across the languages based on the estimate of usage provided. For example, from age 36 to 41 months, child A was cared for by:
-
1.
his mother, who worked 10 h per week: 100% French input: 6 French points
-
2.
his father, who worked 40 h per week: 100% English input: 4 English points
-
3.
part-time (15 h per week) daycare: 75% French, 25% English input, 1.5 French points, 0.5 English points
Points were added for each language (French: 6 + 1.5 = 7.5 points; English: 4 + 0.5 = 4.5 points) and then divided by the total childcare points (12) for that 6 month period to generate a ratio of 62.5% French (7.5/12) and 37.5% English (4.5/12) exposure. All 6 month exposure estimates were subsequently averaged into yearly subtotaled ratios and a summary Lifetime Ratio (LR). The language that received the highest percentage of lifetime exposure was designated the child’s dominant exposure language.
Appendix 2: Questions from the ADI-R: Scoring and Modifications
The ADI-R is a semi-structured interview (Le Couteur et al. 2003). Trained interviewers ask a series of questions to elicit caregiver descriptions of target behaviors; the interviewer categorizes each behavior using the coding schema for each specific question. For complete question and coding information, please consult the ADI-R manual. The following overview summarizes the lead questions and the coding synopses including codes that were merged due to low cell numbers to facilitate statistical analyses.
Overall level of language (ADI-R #30):
Lead interview question: “How much speech does the subject have now?”
Codes of ‘0’ (functional use of phrases) were contrasted against merged codes of ‘1’ (no phrase use but speech used on a daily basis) and ‘2’ (fewer than 5 words total).
Comprehension of simple language (ADI-R #29):
Lead interview question: “How much language do you think s/he understands if you don’t gesture?”
Codes of ‘0’ (phrase-level comprehension; able to perform novel requests) and ‘1’ (phrase-level comprehension; able to perform familiar requests) were merged, as were scores of ‘2’ (word-level comprehension; >50 words understood) and ‘3’ (word-level comprehension; <50 words); three scores of ‘4’ (‘little or no word comprehension’) were excluded from the analyses.
Questions pertaining to pointing (ADI-R #42) were divided for the interview into separate items for pointing initiation and response:
Initiation of pointing to express interest (variable ‘42a’):
Lead interview question: Does your child ever spontaneously point at things around him/her?
Codes of ‘0’ (spontaneous pointing to express interest in distant objects) were contrasted against merged codes of ‘1’ (limited use of pointing) and ‘2’ (little to no pointing).
Response to pointing to express interest (variable ‘42b’)
Lead interview question: When you point at an item that interests you, does your child turn to look at what you are pointing at?
Codes of ‘0’ (spontaneous response to pointing) were contrasted against merged codes of ‘1’ (responds to points when cued) and ‘2’ (little or no response).
Attention to voice (ADI-R #46)
Lead interview questions: If you come into a room and start talking to your child without calling her/his name, what does s/he do?
Codes were analyzed in this study as follows: ‘0’ (usually looks up when spoken to), ‘1’ (inconsistent or brief response when spoken to; may respond to a firm voice), and merged codes ‘2’ (usually does not look up without strong cues) and ‘3’ (rarely responds).
Age of first single words used meaningfully (ADI-R #9)
Lead question: How old was s/he when he first used words meaningfully, apart from ‘mama’ and ‘dada’?
Single word use was specified as repeated and consistent communicative use of a word other than terms for parents. Age in months when the milestone was reached was elicited.
Age of first phrases used meaningfully (ADI-R #10)
Lead question: How old was s/he when s/he first said something that involved putting words together meaningfully (i.e., using two word phrases)?
Phrase use was specified as a two word phrase including a verb; echoed phrases are excluded. Age in months when the milestone was reached was elicited.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Hambly, C., Fombonne, E. The Impact of Bilingual Environments on Language Development in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 42, 1342–1352 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1365-z
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1365-z